Ach, N. (1910). Über den Willensakt und das Temperament. Leipzig: Quelle & Meyer.
Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Anderson, J. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (eds.), Unintended thought(pp. 3–51). London: Guilford Press.
Google Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275,1293–1295.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Bekkering, H. & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13, 370– 374.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Bertelson, P. (1961). Sequential redundancy and speed in a serial two-choice responding task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13, 90–102.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Bertelson, P. (1963). S-R relationships and reaction times to new versus repeated signals in a serial task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 478–484.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Bertelson, P. (1965). Serial choice reaction-time as a function of response versus signal-and-response repetition. Nature, 206, 217–218.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Brebner, J. (1973): S-R compatibility and changes in RT with practice. Acta Psychologica, 37, 93–106.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1998). Descartes' Irrtum: Fühlen, Denken und das menschliche Gehirn.München: dtv.
Google Scholar
Donders, F. C. (1868/1969). On the speed of mental processes (original work published 1868). Acta Psychologica, 30,412– 431.
Google Scholar
Dörner, D., Kreuzig, H. W., Reither, F. & Stäudel, T. (eds.). (1983). Lohhausen. Vom Umgang mit Unbestimmtheit und Komplexität. Bern: Huber.
Google Scholar
Exner, S. (1879). Physiologie der Grosshirnrinde. In L. Hermann (ed.), Handbuch der Physiologie, vol. 2(S. 189–350). Leipzig: Vogel.
Google Scholar
Fagioli, S., Hommel, B. & Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions. Psychological Research, 71, 22–29.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Falmagne, J. C., Cohen, S. P. & Dwivedi, A. (1975). Two-choice reactions as an ordered memory scanning process. In P. M. A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (eds.), Attention and performance, vol. V(pp. 296–344). San Diego: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381–391.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Hick, W. E. (1952). On the rate of gain of information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4,11–26.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Hommel, B. (1993). The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: Evidence for a temporal overlap. Psychological Research, 55, 280– 290.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Hommel, B. (1998a). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Hommel, B. (1998b). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5,183–216.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance, vol. XVIII(pp. 247–273). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Hyman, R. (1953). Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45,188– 196.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Keren, G. & Wagenaar, W. A. (1985) On the psychology of playing Blackjack: Normative and descriptive considerations with implications for decision theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 133–158.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Kirby, N. H. (1980). Sequential effects in choice reaction time. In A. T. Welford (ed.), Reaction times(pp 129–172). London: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Kornblum, S. (1973). Sequential effects in choice reaction time: A tutorial review. In S. Kornblum (ed.), Attention and performance, vol. IV(pp. 259–288). New York: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T. & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus response compatibility: A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253– 270.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Leonard, J. A. (1959). Tactual choice reaction times. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 76–83.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Lhermitte, F. (1983). «Utilization behaviour” and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106, 237–255.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Logan, G. D. & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 1072–1090.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Longstreth, L. E., el-Zahhar, N. & Alcorn, M. B. (1985). Exceptions to Hick's law: Explorations with a response duration measure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114,417–434.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Merkel, J. (1885). Die Zeitlichen Verhältnisse der Wiellensthätigkeit. Philosophische Studien, 2, 73–127.
Google Scholar
Meyer, D. E. & Kieras, D. E. (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, 104, 3–65.
Google Scholar
Mowbray, G. H. & Rhoades, M. U. (1959). On the reduction of choice reaction times with practice. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1,16–23.
Google Scholar
Niemi, P. & Näätänen, R. (1981). Foreperiod and simple reaction time. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 133–162.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Rasmussen, J. (1980). What can be learned from human error reports? In K. D. Duncan, M. M. Gruenberg & D. Wallis (eds.), Changes in working life(pp. 97–113). Chichester: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Rhinehart, L. (1971). Der Würfler.Berlin: Ullstein.
Google Scholar
Robertson, S. I. (1999). Types of thinking. London: Routledge.
Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Hommel, B. (1998b). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5,183–216.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance, vol. XVIII(pp. 247–273). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Hyman, R. (1953). Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45,188– 196.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Keren, G. & Wagenaar, W. A. (1985) On the psychology of playing Blackjack: Normative and descriptive considerations with implications for decision theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 133–158.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Kirby, N. H. (1980). Sequential effects in choice reaction time. In A. T. Welford (ed.), Reaction times(pp 129–172). London: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Kornblum, S. (1973). Sequential effects in choice reaction time: A tutorial review. In S. Kornblum (ed.), Attention and performance, vol. IV(pp. 259–288). New York: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T. & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus response compatibility: A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253– 270.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Leonard, J. A. (1959). Tactual choice reaction times. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 76–83.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Lhermitte, F. (1983). «Utilization behaviour” and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106, 237–255.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Logan, G. D. & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 1072–1090.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Longstreth, L. E., el-Zahhar, N. & Alcorn, M. B. (1985). Exceptions to Hick's law: Explorations with a response duration measure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114,417–434.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Merkel, J. (1885). Die Zeitlichen Verhältnisse der Wiellensthätigkeit. Philosophische Studien, 2, 73–127.
Google Scholar
Meyer, D. E. & Kieras, D. E. (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, 104, 3–65.
Google Scholar
Mowbray, G. H. & Rhoades, M. U. (1959). On the reduction of choice reaction times with practice. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1,16–23.
Google Scholar
Niemi, P. & Näätänen, R. (1981). Foreperiod and simple reaction time. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 133–162.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Rasmussen, J. (1980). What can be learned from human error reports? In K. D. Duncan, M. M. Gruenberg & D. Wallis (eds.), Changes in working life(pp. 97–113). Chichester: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Rhinehart, L. (1971). Der Würfler.Berlin: Ullstein.
Google Scholar
Robertson, S. I. (1999). Types of thinking. London: Routledge.
Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar