Mind the Gap pp 395-426 | Cite as

Human Universals and Primate Symplesiomorphies: Establishing the Lemur Baseline

Chapter

Abstract

Social behavior, culture, and cognition are domains where presumably most human universals exist. Identification of these derived human traits depends and relies on comparisons with other primates, notably chimpanzees. This approach can also be used to reconstruct primate and human behavioral evolution. Accordingly, traits found in both Homo and Pan can be inferred to have existed in their last common ancestor as well. By analogy, traits shared between humans and other primates can be traced back even further down on our family tree. Here, we look at the other side of human universals, i.e., behavioral and cognitive traits of the most basal living primates, which ought to represent the common primate legacy upon which later taxon-specific specializations were built. Specifically, we review studies investigating cognitive abilities and social behavior of the lemuriform primates of Madagascar. The Malagasy lemurs are particularly important for this purpose because they alone, among strepsirrhine primates, have evolved group-living, which characterizes most living haplorrhines. Even though lemurs have relatively smaller brains than New and Old World monkeys and great apes, their ability to solve problems that require technical intelligence is qualitatively on par with that of haplorrhines. In the domain of social intelligence, however, lemurs deviate from the better-known haplorrhine models (i.e., cercopithecines) in several respects. Most importantly, their behavioral strategies reflect an emphasis on within-group competition, rather than cooperation, which may represent lemur-specific adaptations to an ecologically unpredictable environment, rather than fundamental deficits in social intelligence. In any event, a broad comparative perspective including the best living models of the earliest gregarious primates can enrich reconstructions and other evolutionary analyzes of primate social behavior, including that of humans.

References

  1. Amici F, Aureli F, Call J (2008) Fission-fusion dynamics, behavioral flexibility, and inhibitory control in primates. Curr Biol 18:1415–1419PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson JR (1983) Optimal foraging and the traveling salesman. Theor Popul Biol 24:145–159Google Scholar
  3. Anderson JR (2001) Self- and other-control in squirrel monkeys. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo, pp 330–347Google Scholar
  4. Anderson JR, Mitchell RW (1999) Macaques but not lemurs co-orient visually with humans. Folia Primatol 70:17–22PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Anderson JR, Fornasieri I, Ludes E, Roeder J-J (1992) Social processes and innovative behaviour in changing groups of Lemur fulvus. Behav Process 27:101–112Google Scholar
  6. Anderson JR, Awazu S, Fujita K (2000) Can squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) learn self-control? A study using food array selection tests and reverse-reward contingency. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 26:87–97PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Andrews J (1998) Infanticide by a female black lemur, Eulemur macaco, in disturbed habitat on Nosy Be, North-Western Madagascar. Folia Primatol 69(Suppl 1):14–17PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Arbib MA, Liebal K, Pika S (2008) Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language. Curr Anthropol 49:1053–1076PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Armstrong E (1985) Relative brain size in monkeys and prosimians. Am J Phys Anthropol 66:263–273PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Aureli F, de Waal FBM (2000) Natural conflict resolution. University of California Press, BerkelyGoogle Scholar
  11. Barrett L, Henzi SP, Weingrill T, Lycett JE, Hill RA (1999) Market forces predict grooming reciprocity in female baboons. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 266:665–670Google Scholar
  12. Barthold J, Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2009) What is it going to be? Pattern and potential function of natal coat change in sexually dichromatic redfronted lemurs (Eulemur fulvus rufus). Am J Phys Anthropol 138:1–10PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Benadi G, Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2008) Intergroup relations and home range use in Verreaux’s sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi). Am J Primatol 70:956–965PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Beran MJ, Beran MM (2004) Chimpanzees remember the results of one-by-one addition of food items to sets over extended time periods. Psychol Sci 15:94–99PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Birkinshaw CR (1999) Use of millipedes by black lemurs to anoint their bodies. Folia Primatol 70:170–171PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Boesch C (2007) What makes us human (Homo sapiens)? The challenge of cognitive cross-species comparison. J Comp Psychol 121:227–240PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Boesch C, Boesch H (1984) Mental map in wild chimpanzees: an analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking. Primates 25:160–170Google Scholar
  18. Boinski S, Garber PA (2000) On the move: how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  19. Bond AB, Kamil AC, Balda RP (2003) Social complexity and transitive inference in corvids. Anim Behav 65:479–487Google Scholar
  20. Boysen ST, Berntson GG (1995) Responses to quantity: perceptual versus cognitive mechanisms in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 21:82–86PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Bradley BJ (2008) Reconstructing phylogenies and phenotypes: a molecular view of human evolution. J Anat 212:337–353PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Brannon E, Terrace HS (2000) Representation of the numerosities 1–9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 26:31–49PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Bräuer J, Call J, Tomasello M (2005) All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and around barriers. J Comp Psychol 119:145–154PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Brosnan SF, de Waal FBM (2003) Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature 425:297–299PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Byrne RW, Corp N (2004) Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 271:1693–1699Google Scholar
  26. Call J (2001) Object permanence in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and children (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 115:159–171PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Call J, Agnetta B, Tomasello M (2000) Cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden objects. Anim Cogn 3:23–34Google Scholar
  28. Call J, Bräuer J, Kaminski J, Tomasello M (2003) Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. J Comp Psychol 117:257–263PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Charles-Dominique P (1977) Ecology and behaviour of nocturnal primates. Columbia University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  30. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1990) How monkeys see the world. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  31. Cook M, Mineka S (1989) Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. J Abnorm Psychol 98:448–459PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Cools AKA, van Hout AJM, Nelissen MHJ (2008) Canine reconciliation and third-party-initiated postconflict affiliation: do peacemaking social mechanisms in dogs rival those of higher primates? Ethology 114:53–63Google Scholar
  33. Cooper H (1974) Learning set in Lemur macaco. In: Martin RD, Doyle GA, Walker AC (eds) Prosimian biology. University of Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, PA, pp 293–300Google Scholar
  34. Cummins-Sebree SE, Fragaszy D (2001) The right stuff: capuchin monkeys perceive affordances of tools. In: Burton G, Schmidt R (eds) Studies in perception and action, vol VI. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NY, pp 89–92Google Scholar
  35. Curio E, Ernst U, Vieth W (1978) Cultural transmission of enemy recognition: one function of mobbing. Science 202:899–901PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. D’Amato MR, Colombo M (1989) Serial learning with wild card items by monkeys (Cebus apella): implications for knowledge of ordinal position. J Comp Psychol 103:252–261PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Dammhahn M, Kappeler PM (2008) Small-scale coexistence of two mouse lemur species (Microcebus berthae and M. murinus) within a homogeneous competitive environment. Oecologia 157:473–483PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Davis RT, Leary RW (1968) Learning of detour problems by lemurs and seven species of monkeys. Percept Mot Skills 27:1031–1034Google Scholar
  39. Davis RT, Leary RW, Stevens DA, Thompson RF (1967) Learning and perception of oddity problems by lemurs and seven species of monkeys. Primates 8:311–322Google Scholar
  40. de Blois ST, Novak MA, Bond M (1998) Object permanence in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). J Comp Psychol 112:137–152PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Deaner RO, van Schaik CP, Johnson V (2006) Do some taxa have better domain-general cognition than others? A meta-analysis of nonhuman primate studies. Evol Psychol 4:149–196Google Scholar
  42. Deaner RO, Isler K, Burkart J, van Schaik CP (2007) Overall brain size, and not encephalization quotient, best predicts cognitive ability across non-human primates. Brain Behav Evol 70:115–124PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Deppe AM, Wright PC, Szelistowski WA (2009) Object permanence in lemurs. Anim Cogn 12:381–388PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Dewar RE, Richard AF (2007) Evolution in the hypervariable environment of Madagascar. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:13723–13727PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Digby LJ, Haley M, Schneider AC, Del Valle I (2008) Sensorimotor intelligence in aye-ayes and other lemurs: a puzzle box approach. Am J Primatol 70(Suppl 1):54Google Scholar
  46. Dunbar RIM (1995) Neocortex size and group size in primates: a test of the hypothesis. J Hum Evol 28:287–296Google Scholar
  47. Dunbar RIM (1998) The social brain hypothesis. Evol Anthropol 6:178–190Google Scholar
  48. Dunbar RIM, Shultz S (2007) Understanding primate brain evolution. Phil Trans Roy Soc B 362:649–658Google Scholar
  49. Eberle M, Kappeler PM (2006) Family insurance: kin selection and cooperative breeding in a solitary primate (Microcebus murinus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60:582–588Google Scholar
  50. Eberle M, Kappeler PM (2008) Mutualism, reciprocity, or kin selection? Cooperative rescue of a conspecific from a boa in a nocturnal solitary forager the gray mouse lemur. Am J Primatol 70:410–414PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Ehrlich A (1970) Response to novel objects in three lower primates: greater galago, slow loris, and owl monkey. Behaviour 37:55–63Google Scholar
  52. Ehrlich A, Fobes JL, King JE (1976) Prosimian learning capacities. J Hum Evol 5:599–617Google Scholar
  53. Erhart EM, Overdorff DJ (2008) Spatial memory during foraging in prosimian primates: Propithecus edwardsi and Eulemur fulvus rufus. Folia Primatol 79:185–196PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Erickson CJ (1995) Feeding sites for extractive foraging by the aye-aye, Daubentonia madagascariensis. Am J Primatol 35:235–240Google Scholar
  55. Erkert HG, Kappeler PM (2004) Arrived in the light: diel and seasonal activity patterns in wild Verreaux’s sifakas (Propithecus v. verreauxi; Primates: Indriidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57:174–186Google Scholar
  56. Evans TA, Beran MJ, Harris EH, Rice DF (2009) Quantity judgments of sequentially presented food items by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Anim Cogn 12:97–105PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Feigenson L, Carey S, Spelke E (2002) Infants’ discrimination of number vs. continuous extent. Cogn Psychol 44:33–66PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Feldman DW, Klopfer PH (1972) A study of observational learning in lemurs. Z Tierpsychol 30:297–304Google Scholar
  59. Fichtel C (2007) Avoiding predators at night: antipredator strategies in red-tailed sportive lemurs (Lepilemur ruficaudatus). Am J Primatol 69:611–624PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Fichtel C (2008) Ontogeny of conspecific and heterospecific alarm call recognition in wild Verreaux’s sifakas (Propithecus v. verreauxi). Am J Primatol 70:127–135PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Fichtel C, Hammerschmidt K (2002) Responses of redfronted lemurs (Eulemur fulvus rufus) to experimentally modified alarm calls: evidence for urgency-based changes in call structure. Ethology 108:763–777Google Scholar
  62. Fichtel C, Hammerschmidt K (2003) Responses of squirrel monkeys to their experimentally modified mobbing calls. J Acoust Soc Am 113:2927–2932PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2002) Anti-predator behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed evidence for a referential alarm call system. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:262–275Google Scholar
  64. Fichtel C, van Schaik CP (2006) Semantic differences in sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi) alarm calls: a reflection of genetic or cultural variants? Ethology 112:839–849Google Scholar
  65. Fichtel C, Hammerschmidt K, Jürgens J (2001) On the vocal expression of emotion. A multi-parametric analysis of different states of aversion in the squirrel monkey. Behaviour 138:97–116Google Scholar
  66. Fichtel C, Perry S, Gros-Louis J (2005) Alarm calls of white-faced capuchin monkeys: an acoustic analysis. Anim Behav 70:165–176Google Scholar
  67. Fobes JL, King JE (1982) Measuring primate learning abilities. In: Fobes JL, King JE (eds) Primate behavior. Academic, New York, pp 289–326Google Scholar
  68. Fogassi L, Ferrari PF (2007) Mirror neurons and the evolution of embodied language. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16:136–141Google Scholar
  69. Fornasieri I, Anderson JR, Roeder J-J (1990) Responses to a novel food acquisition task in three species of lemurs. Behav Process 21:143–156Google Scholar
  70. Fujita K, Kuroshima H, Asai S (2003) How do tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) understand causality involved in tool use? J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 29:233–242PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Gallistel CR (1989) Animal cognition: the representation of space, time and number. Annu Rev Psychol 40:155–189PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Garber PA (1988) Foraging decisions during nectar feeding by tamarin monkeys (Saguinus mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis, Callitrichidae, Primates) in Amazonian Peru. Biotropica 20:100–106Google Scholar
  73. Genty E, Roeder J-J (2006) Can lemurs learn to deceive? A study in the black lemurs (Eulemur macaco). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 32:196–200PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Genty E, Palmier C, Roeder J-J (2004) Learning to suppress responses to the larger of two rewards in two species of lemurs, Eulemur fulvus and E. macaco. Anim Behav 67:925–932Google Scholar
  75. Genty E, Foltz J, Roeder J-J (2008) Can brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus) learn to deceive a human competitor? Anim Cogn 11:255–266PubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Glander KE, Freed BZ, Ganzhorn JU (1985) Meat eating and predation in captive-born semi-free-ranging Lemur fulvus and caged Lemur macaco. Zoo Biol 4:361–365Google Scholar
  77. Gosset D, Roeder J-J (2001) Factors affecting feeding decisions in a group of black lemurs confronted with novel food. Primates 42:175–182Google Scholar
  78. Hammerschmidt K, Jürgens U (2007) Acoustical correlates of affective prosody. J Voice 21:531–540PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Hammerschmidt K, Freudenstein T, Jürgens U (2001) Vocal development in squirrel monkeys. Behaviour 138:1179–1204Google Scholar
  80. Hanus D, Call J (2007) Discrete quantity judgments in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus): the effect of presenting whole sets versus item-by-item. J Comp Psychol 121:241–249PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Hare B, Tomasello M (2004) Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Anim Behav 68:571–581Google Scholar
  82. Harlow HF (1949) The formation of learning sets. Psychol Rev 56:51–65PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Harlow HF, Uehling H, Maslow AH (1932) Comparative behavior of primates. I. Delayed reaction tests on primates from the lemur to the orangutan. J Comp Psychol 13:313–343Google Scholar
  84. Hauser MD (2000) Wild minds: what animals really think. Holt, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  85. Hauser MD (2001) Searching for food in the wild: a nonhuman primate’s expectations about invisible displacement. Dev Sci 4:84–93Google Scholar
  86. Hauser MD, MacNeilage P, Ware M (1996) Numerical representations in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:1514–1517PubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Hauser MD, Kralik J, Botto-Mahan C (1999) Problem solving and functional design features: experiments on cotton-top tamarins, Saguinus oedipus. Anim Behav 57:565–582PubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Hauser MD, Santos LR, Spaepen GM, Pearson HE (2002) Problem solving, inhibition and domain-specific experience: experiments on cottontop tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Anim Behav 64:387–396Google Scholar
  89. Hauser MD, Tsao F, Garcia P, Spelke ES (2003) Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representation of numerical magnitudes by cotton-top tamarins. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 270:1441–1446Google Scholar
  90. Henzi SP, Barrett L (1999) The value of grooming to female primates. Primates 40:47–59Google Scholar
  91. Horvath JE, Weisrock DW, Embry SL, Fiorentino I, Balhoff JP, Kappeler PM, Wray GA, Willard HF, Yoder AD (2008) Development and application of a phylogenomic toolkit: resolving the evolutionary history of Madagascar’s lemurs. Genome Res 18:489–499PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Hosey GR, Jacques M, Pitts A (1997) Drinking from tails: social learning of a novel behaviour in a group of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Primates 38:415–422Google Scholar
  93. Itakura S (1996) An exploratory study of gaze monitoring in non-human primates. Jpn Psychol Res 38:174–180Google Scholar
  94. Janson CH (1998) Experimental evidence for spatial memory in foraging wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Anim Behav 55:1229–1243PubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Jolly A (1964a) Choice of cue in prosimian learning. Anim Behav 12:571–577Google Scholar
  96. Jolly A (1964b) Prosimians’ manipulation of simple object problems. Anim Behav 12:560–570Google Scholar
  97. Jolly A (1966a) Lemur behavior. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  98. Jolly A (1966b) Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence. Science 153:501–506PubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Jolly A, Oliver WLR (1985) Predatory behavior in captive Lemur spp. Zoo Biol 4:139–145Google Scholar
  100. Jolly A, Caless S, Cavigelli S, Gould L, Pereira ME, Pitts A, Pride RE, Rabenandrasana HD, Walker JD, Zafison T (2000) Infant killing, wounding and predation in Eulemur and Lemur. Int J Primatol 21:21–40Google Scholar
  101. Joly M, Zimmermann E (2007) First evidence for relocation of stationary food resources during foraging in a strepsirrhine primate (Microcebus murinus). Am J Primatol 69:1045–1052PubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Jones KC (1983) Inter-troop transfer of Lemur catta males at Berenty, Madagascar. Folia Primatol 40:145–160PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Kaminski J, Riedel J, Call J, Tomasello M (2005) Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use social cues in an object choice task. Anim Behav 69:11–18Google Scholar
  104. Kappeler PM (1987) The acquisition process of a novel behavior pattern in a group of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Primates 28:225–228Google Scholar
  105. Kappeler PM (1993a) Reconciliation and post-conflict behaviour in ringtailed lemurs, Lemur catta and redfronted lemurs, Eulemur fulvus rufus. Anim Behav 45:901–915Google Scholar
  106. Kappeler PM (1993b) Variation in social structure: the effects of sex and kinship on social interactions in three lemur species. Ethology 93:125–145Google Scholar
  107. Kappeler PM (1998) To whom it may concern: the transmission and function of chemical signals in Lemur catta. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:411–4421Google Scholar
  108. Kappeler PM (1999) Convergence and divergence in primate social systems. In: Fleagle JG, Janson CH, Reed K (eds) Primate communities. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 158–170Google Scholar
  109. Kappeler PM (2000) Causes and consequences of unusual sex ratios among lemurs. In: Kappeler PM (ed) Primate males: causes and consequences of variation in group composition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 55–63Google Scholar
  110. Kappeler PM, Erkert HG (2003) On the move around the clock: correlates and determinants of cathemeral activity in wild redfronted lemurs (Eulemur fulvus rufus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 54:359–369Google Scholar
  111. Kappeler PM, Heymann EW (1996) Nonconvergence in the evolution of primate life history and socio-ecology. Biol J Linn Soc 59:297–326Google Scholar
  112. Kappeler PM, van Schaik CP (2002) Evolution of primate social systems. Int J Primatol 23:707–740Google Scholar
  113. Kappeler PM, Wimmer B, Zinner DP, Tautz D (2002) The hidden matrilineal structure of a solitary lemur: implications for primate social evolution. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 269:1755–1763Google Scholar
  114. Karanth KP, Delefosse T, Rakotosamimanana B, Parsons TJ, Yoder AD (2005) Ancient DNA from giant extinct lemurs confirms single origin of Malagasy primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:5090–5095PubMedGoogle Scholar
  115. Kirchhof J, Hammerschmidt K (2006) Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax) – evidence from playback experiments. Ethology 112:346–354Google Scholar
  116. Krakauer EB (2005) Development of aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis) foraging skills: independent exploration and social learning. PhD-thesis, Duke University, Durham, NCGoogle Scholar
  117. Kralik JD, Hauser MD, Zimlicki R (2002) The relationship between problem solving and inhibitory control: cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus) performance on a reversed contingency task. J Comp Psychol 116:39–50PubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Kudo H, Dunbar RIM (2001) Neocortex size and social network size in primates. Anim Behav 62:711–722Google Scholar
  119. Laland KN (2004) Social learning strategies. Learn Behav 32:4–14PubMedGoogle Scholar
  120. Lewis KP (2000) A comparative study of primate play behaviour: implications for the study of cognition. Folia Primatol 71:417–421PubMedGoogle Scholar
  121. Lewis KP, Jaffe S, Brannon EM (2005) Analog number representations in mongoose lemurs (Eulemur mongoz): evidence from a search task. Anim Cogn 8:247–252PubMedGoogle Scholar
  122. Lhota S, Jůnek T, Bartoš L, Kuběna AA (2008) Specialized use of two fingers in free-ranging aye-ayes (Daubentonia madagascariensis). Am J Primatol 70:786–795PubMedGoogle Scholar
  123. Lindenfors P (2005) Neocortex evolution in primates: the ‘social brain’ is for females. Biol Lett 1:407–410PubMedGoogle Scholar
  124. Lührs M-L, Dammhahn M, Kappeler PM, Fichtel C (2009) Spatial memory in the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus). Anim Cogn. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0219-y PubMedGoogle Scholar
  125. Macedonia JM (1990) What is communicated in the antipredator calls of lemurs: evidence from playback experiments with ringtailed and ruffed lemurs. Ethology 86:177–190Google Scholar
  126. MacLean EL, Merritt DJ, Brannon EM (2008) Social complexity predicts transitive reasoning in prosimian primates. Anim Behav 76:479–486PubMedGoogle Scholar
  127. Martin RD, Harvey PH (1985) Brain size allometry: ontogeny and phylogeny. In: Jungers WL (ed) Size and scaling in primate biology. Plenum Press, New York, pp 147–173Google Scholar
  128. Martin-Ordas G, Call J, Colmenares F (2008) Tubes, tables and traps: great apes solve two functionally equivalent trap tasks but show no evidence of transfer across tasks. Anim Cogn 11:423–430PubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Maslow AH, Harlow HF (1932) Comparative behavior of primates. II. Delayed reaction tests on primates at Bronx Park Zoo. J Comp Psychol 14:97–107Google Scholar
  130. Meador DM, Rumbaugh DM, Pate JL, Bard KA (1987) Learning, problem solving, cognition, and intelligence. In: Mitchell G, Erwin J (eds) Comparative primate biology: behavior, cognition, and motivation. Alan R Liss, New York, pp 17–83Google Scholar
  131. Mendes N, Huber L (2004) Object permanence in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). J Comp Psychol 118:103–112PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Mendes N, Hanus D, Call J (2007) Raising the level: orangutans use water as a tool. Biol Lett 3:453–455PubMedGoogle Scholar
  133. Menzel EW Jr (1973) Leadership and communication in young chimpanzees. In: Menzel EW Jr (ed) Precultural primate behavior. Karger, Basel, pp 192–225Google Scholar
  134. Menzel CR (1991) Cognitive aspects of foraging in Japanese monkeys. Anim Behav 41:397–402Google Scholar
  135. Merritt D, Maclean EL, Jaffe S, Brannon EM (2007) A comparative analysis of serial ordering in ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). J Comp Psychol 121:363–371PubMedGoogle Scholar
  136. Mikkelsen TS, Hillier LW, Eichler EE, Zody MC, Jaffe DB, Yang S-P, Enard W, Hellmann I, Lindblad-Toh K, Altheide TK, Archidiacono N, Bork P, Butler J, Chang JL, Cheng Z, Chinwalla AT, de Jong P, Delehaunty KD, Fronick CC, Fulton LL, Gilad Y, Glusman G, Gnerre S, Graves TA, Hayakawa T, Hayden KE, Huang X, Ji H, Kent WJ, King M-C, Kulbokas EJ, Lee MK, Liu G, Lopez-Otin C, Makova KD, Man O, Mardis ER, Mauceli E, Miner TL, Nash WE, Nelson JO, Pääbo S, Patterson NJ, Pohl CS, Pollard KS, Prüfer K, Puente XS, Reich D, Rocchi M, Rosenbloom K, Ruvolo M, Richter DJ, Schaffner SF, Smit AFA, Smith SM, Suyama M, Taylor J, Torrents D, Tuzun E, Varki A, Velasco G, Ventura M, Wallis JW, Wendl MC, Wilson RK, Lander ES, Waterston RH (2005) Initial sequence of the chimpanzee genome and comparison with the human genome. Nature 437:69–87Google Scholar
  137. Miliken GW, Ward JP, Erickson CJ (1991) Independent digit control in foraging by the aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis). Folia Primatol 56:219–224Google Scholar
  138. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez ML (1989) Delay of gratification in children. Science 244:933–938PubMedGoogle Scholar
  139. Mitchell RW, Thompson NS (1986) Deception: perspectives on human and nonhuman deceit. SUNY Press, AlbanyGoogle Scholar
  140. Moura AC, Lee PC (2004) Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga dry forest. Science 306:1909PubMedGoogle Scholar
  141. Neiworth JJ, Steinmark E, Basile BM, Wonders R, Steely F, DeHart C (2003) A test of object permanence in a new-world monkey species, cotton top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Anim Cogn 6:27–37PubMedGoogle Scholar
  142. Noser R, Byrne RW (2007) Travel routes and planning of visits to out-of-sight resources in wild chacma baboons, Papio ursinus. Anim Behav 73:257–266Google Scholar
  143. Nunn CL, Deaner RO (2004) Patterns of participation and free riding in territorial conflicts among ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57:50–61Google Scholar
  144. Ohta H (1983) Learning set formation in slow lorises (Nycticebus coucang). Folia Primatol 40:256–267PubMedGoogle Scholar
  145. Ohta H, Ishida H, Matano S (1984) Learning set formation in ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Folia Primatol 43:53–58Google Scholar
  146. Ohta H, Ishida H, Matano S (1987) Learning set formation in thick-tailed bush babies (Galago crassicaudatus) and comparison of learning ability among four species. Folia Primatol 48:1–8Google Scholar
  147. Ostner J, Kappeler PM (2004) Male life history and the unusal adult sex ratios of redfronted lemur, Eulemur fulvus rufus, groups. Anim Behav 67:249–259Google Scholar
  148. Overdorff DJ, Merenlender AM, Talata P, Telo A, Forward ZA (1999) Life history of Eulemur fulvus rufus from 1988–1998 in Southeastern Madagascar. Am J Phys Anthropol 108:295–310PubMedGoogle Scholar
  149. Palagi E, Paoli T, Borgognini Tarli SM (2005) Aggression and reconciliation in two captive groups of Lemur catta. Int J Primatol 26:279–294Google Scholar
  150. Palagi E, Antonacci D, Norscia I (2008) Peacemaking on treetops: first evidence of reconciliation from a wild prosimian (Propithecus verreauxi). Anim Behav 76:737–747Google Scholar
  151. Parker CE (1973) Manipulatory behavior and responsiveness. In: Rumbaugh DM (ed) Gibbon and Siamang, vol 2, Anatyomy, dentition, taxonomy, molecular evolution and behavior. Karger, Basel, pp 185–207Google Scholar
  152. Parker CE (1974) Behavioural diversity in ten species of nonhuman primates. J Comp Physiol Psychol 87:930–937PubMedGoogle Scholar
  153. Pereira ME (1993) Agonistic interaction, dominance relation, and ontogenetic trajectories in ringtailed lemurs. In: Pereira ME, Fairbanks LA (eds) Juvenile primates: life history, development, and behavior. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 285–305Google Scholar
  154. Pereira ME, Kappeler PM (1997) Divergent systems of agonistic behaviour in lemurid primates. Behaviour 134:225–274Google Scholar
  155. Pereira ME, Macedonia JM (1991) Ringtailed lemur anti-predator calls denote predator class, not response urgency. Anim Behav 41:543–544Google Scholar
  156. Pereira ME, Kaufman R, Kappeler PM, Overdorff DJ (1990) Female dominance does not characterize all of the Lemuridae. Folia Primatol 55:96–103PubMedGoogle Scholar
  157. Perry S, Manson JH (2003) Traditions in monkeys. Evol Anthropol 12:71–81Google Scholar
  158. Picq JL (1993) Radial maze performance in young and aged grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus). Primates 24:223–226Google Scholar
  159. Picq JL (2007) Aging affects executive functions and memory in mouse lemur primates. Exp Gerontol 42:223–232PubMedGoogle Scholar
  160. Pochron ST, Fitzgerald J, Gilbert CC, Lawrence D, Grgas M, Rakotonirina G, Ratsimbazafy R, Rakotosoa R, Wright PC (2003) Patterns of female dominance in Propithecus diadema edwardsi of Ranomafana National Park, Madagascar. Am J Primatol 61:173–185PubMedGoogle Scholar
  161. Pochron ST, Tucker WT, Wright PC (2004) Demography, life history, and social structure in Propithecus diadema edwardsi from 1986–2000 in Ranomafana National Park, Madagascar. Am J Phys Anthropol 125:61–72PubMedGoogle Scholar
  162. Pochron ST, Morelli TL, Scirbona J, Wright PC (2005) Sex differences in scent-marking in Propithecus edwardsi of Ranomafana National Park, Madagascar. Am J Primatol 66:97–110PubMedGoogle Scholar
  163. Pollick AS, de Waal FBM (2007) Ape gestures and language evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:8184–8189PubMedGoogle Scholar
  164. Port M, Clough D, Kappeler PM (2009) Market effects offset the reciprocation of grooming in free-ranging red-fronted lemurs, Eulemur fulvus rufus. Anim Behav 77:29–36Google Scholar
  165. Povinelli DJ (2000) Folk physics for apes: the chimpanzee’s theory of how the world works. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  166. Reader SM, Laland KN (2002) Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:4436–4441PubMedGoogle Scholar
  167. Renner MJ, Bennett AJ, Ford ML, Pierre PJ (1992) Investigation of inanimate objects by the greater bushbaby (Otolemur garnettii). Primates 33:315–327Google Scholar
  168. Richard AF (1987) Malagasy prosimians: female dominance. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 25–33Google Scholar
  169. Richard AF, Dewar RE (1991) Lemur ecology. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 22:145–175Google Scholar
  170. Richard AF, Rakotomanga P, Schwartz M (1993) Dispersal by Propithecus verreauxi at Beza Mahafaly, Madagascar: 1984–1991. Am J Primatol 30:1–20Google Scholar
  171. Roeder J-J, Fornasieri I, Gosset D (2002) Conflict and postconflict behaviour in two lemur species with different social organizations (Eulemur fulvus and Eulemur macaco): a study on captive groups. Aggress Behav 28:62–74Google Scholar
  172. Rolland N, Roeder J-J (2000) Do ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta) reconcile in the hour post-conflict? A pilot study. Primates 41:223–227Google Scholar
  173. Rugani R, Fontanari L, Simoni E, Regolin L, Vallortigara G (2009) Arithmetic in newborn chicks. Proc Roy Soc Lond B. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.0044 Google Scholar
  174. Ruiz A, Gómez JC, Roeder J-J, Byrne RW (2009) Gaze following and gaze priming in lemurs. Anim Cogn 12:427–434PubMedGoogle Scholar
  175. Rumbough DM (1970) Learning skills of anthropoids. In: Rosenblum LA (ed) Primate behavior: developments in field and laboratory research, vol 1. Academic, New York, pp 1–70Google Scholar
  176. Rumbough DM (1997) Competence, cortex, and primate models: a comparative primate perspective. In: Krasnegor NA, Lyon GR, Goldman-Rakic PS (eds) Development of the prefontal cortex: evolution, neurobiology and behavior. Paul H Brookes, Baltimore, MD, pp 117–139Google Scholar
  177. Sands SF, Wright AA (1980) Primate memory: retention of serial list items by a rhesus monkey. Science 209:938–940PubMedGoogle Scholar
  178. Santos LR, Mahajan N, Barnes JL (2005a) How prosimian primates represent tools: experiments with two lemur species (Eulemur fulvus and Lemur catta). J Comp Psychol 119:394–403PubMedGoogle Scholar
  179. Santos LR, Barnes JL, Mahajan N (2005b) Expectations about numerical events in four lemur species (Eulemur fulvus, Eulemur mongoz, Lemur catta and Varecia rubra). Anim Cogn 8:253–262PubMedGoogle Scholar
  180. Santos LR, Pearson HM, Spaepen GM, Tsao F, Hauser MD (2006) Probing the limits of tool competence: experiments with two non-tool-using species (Cercopithecus aethiops and Saguinus oedipus). Anim Cogn 9:94–109PubMedGoogle Scholar
  181. Scheiner E, Hammerschmidt K, Jürgens U, Zwirner P (2002) Acoustic analyses of developmental changes and emotional expressions in the preverbal vocalizations of infants. J Voice 16:509–529PubMedGoogle Scholar
  182. Scheiner E, Hammerschmidt K, Jürgens U, Zwirner P (2006) Vocal expression of emotions in normally hearing and hearing-impaired infants. J Voice 20:585–604PubMedGoogle Scholar
  183. Schilling A (2007) Cognitive capacities studied through object manipulation in the prosimian primate Microcebus murinus. Proceedings of the international prosimian conference, Ithala, p 67Google Scholar
  184. Schumaker RW, Palkovich AM, Beck BB, Guagnano GA, Morowitz H (2001) Spontaneous use of magnitude discrimination and ordination by the orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 115:385–391Google Scholar
  185. Seed AM, Call J, Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2009) Chimpanzees solve the trap problem when the confound of tool-use is removed. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 35:23–34PubMedGoogle Scholar
  186. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1980) The ontogeny of vervet monkey alarm calling behavior: a preliminary report. Z Tierpsychol 54:37–56Google Scholar
  187. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1984) Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature 308:541–543PubMedGoogle Scholar
  188. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2003) Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Annu Rev Psychol 54:145–173PubMedGoogle Scholar
  189. Shepherd SV, Platt ML (2008) Spontaneous social orienting and gaze following in ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Anim Cogn 11:13–20PubMedGoogle Scholar
  190. Shultz S, Dunbar RIM (2007) The evolution of the social brain: anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 274:2429–2436Google Scholar
  191. Silberberg A, Fujita K (1996) Pointing at smaller food amounts in an analogue of Boysen and Berntson’s (1995) procedure. J Exp Anal Behav 66:143–147PubMedGoogle Scholar
  192. Silk JB (2007) Social components of fitness in primate groups. Science 317:1347–1351PubMedGoogle Scholar
  193. Spaulding B, Hauser MD (2005) What experience is required for acquiring tool competence? Experiments with two callitrichids. Anim Behav 70:517–526Google Scholar
  194. Stephan H, Baron G, Frahm HD (1988) Comparative size of brains and brain components. In: Steklis HD, Erwin J (eds) Comparative primate biology. Alan R Liss, New York, pp 1–38Google Scholar
  195. Sterling EJ (1994) Aye-ayes: specialists on structurally defended resources. Folia Primatol 62:142–154PubMedGoogle Scholar
  196. Sterling EJ, Povinelli DJ (1999) Tool use, aye-ayes, and sensorimotor intelligence. Folia Primatol 70:8–16PubMedGoogle Scholar
  197. Stevens DA (1965) A comparison of learning in rhesus monkeys, cebus monkeys, lemurs, and Burmese cats. PhD-thesis, University of OregonGoogle Scholar
  198. Straub RO, Terrace HS (1981) Generalization of serial learning in the pigeon. Anim Learn Behav 9:454–468Google Scholar
  199. Strier KB (1994) Myth of the typical primate. Yrbk Phys Anthropol 37:233–271Google Scholar
  200. Sussman RW (1992) Male life history and intergroup mobility among ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Int J Primatol 13:395–413Google Scholar
  201. Tattersall I (2007) Madagascar’s lemurs: cryptic diversity or taxonomic inflation? Evol Anthropol 16:12–23Google Scholar
  202. Terrace HS, Son LK, Brannon EM (2003) Serial expertise of rhesus macaques. Psychol Sci 14:66–73PubMedGoogle Scholar
  203. Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate cognition. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  204. Tomasello M, Call J, Hare B (1998) Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics. Anim Behav 55:1063–1069PubMedGoogle Scholar
  205. Torigoe T (1985) Comparison of object manipulation among 74 species of non-human primates. Primates 26:182–194Google Scholar
  206. Trillmich J, Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2004) Coordination of group movements in wild Verreaux’s sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi). Behaviour 141:1103–1120Google Scholar
  207. Uller C, Hauser MD, Carey S (2001) Spontaneous representation of number in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). J Comp Psychol 115:248–257PubMedGoogle Scholar
  208. van Schaik CP, Kappeler PM (1996) The social systems of gregarious lemurs: lack of convergence with anthropoids due to evolutionary disequilibrium? Ethology 102:915–941Google Scholar
  209. van Schaik CP, Deaner RO, Merrill MY (1999) The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture. J Hum Evol 36:719–741PubMedGoogle Scholar
  210. van Schaik CP, Ancrenaz M, Borgen G, Galdikas B, Knott CD, Singleton I, Suzuki A, Utami SS, Merrill M (2003) Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science 299:102–105PubMedGoogle Scholar
  211. Vick LG, Pereira ME (1989) Episodic targeting aggression and the histories of Lemur social groups. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 25:3–12Google Scholar
  212. Visalberghi E (1993) Capuchin monkeys: a window into tool use in apes and humans. In: Gibson KR, Ingold T (eds) Tools, language and cognition in human evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 138–150Google Scholar
  213. Ward JP, Silver BV, Frank J (1976) Preservation of cross-modal transfer of a rate discrimination in the bushbaby (Galago senegalensis) with lesions of posterior neocortex. J Comp Physiol Psychol 90:520–527PubMedGoogle Scholar
  214. Wheeler BC (2008) Selfish or altruistic? An analysis of alarm call function in wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus. Anim Behav 76:1465–1475Google Scholar
  215. Whiten A, Byrne RW (1997) Machiavellian intelligence. II. Extensions and evaluations. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  216. Whiten A, van Schaik CP (2007) The evolution of animal ‘cultures’ and social intelligence. Phil Trans Roy Soc B 362:603–620Google Scholar
  217. Whiten A, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nishida T, Reynolds V, Sugiyama Y, Tutin CEG, Wrangham RW, Boesch C (1999) Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399:682–685PubMedGoogle Scholar
  218. Wimmer B, Tautz D, Kappeler PM (2002) The genetic population structure of the gray mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus), a basal primate from Madagascar. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 52:166–175Google Scholar
  219. Winter P, Handley P, Ploog D, Schott D (1973) Ontogeny of squirrel monkey calls under normal conditions and under acoustic isolation. Behaviour 47:230–239PubMedGoogle Scholar
  220. Wright PC (1997) Behavioral and ecological comparisons of neotropical and Malagasy primates. In: Kinzey WG (ed) New world primates: ecology, evolution, and behavior. Aldine de Gruyter, New York, pp 127–141Google Scholar
  221. Wright PC (1999) Lemur traits and Madagascar ecology: coping with an island environment. Ybk Phys Anthropol 42:31–72Google Scholar
  222. Wynn K (1992) Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature 358:749–750PubMedGoogle Scholar
  223. Yoder AD (2007) Lemurs. Curr Biol 17:R866–R868PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Behavioral Ecology & SociobiologyGerman Primate CenterGöttingenGermany
  2. 2.Department of Sociobiology/AnthropologyUniversity of GöttingenGöttingenGermany

Personalised recommendations