Skip to main content

Fundamental Motives and Business Decisions

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

You walk into a crowded negotiation room. Who do you notice? Who do you later remember? Do you try to fit in, or attempt to stand out from others? Do you accept the first reasonable offer, or do you balk at that offer? The answers likely depend critically on your current motivational state. Emerging evidence shows that a person’s behavior differs—sometimes dramatically—depending on whether that person is concerned with personal safety, romance, status-seeking, affiliation, or is motivated to attain some other evolutionary important goal. A growing body of research suggests that certain motivational states are considered “fundamental” in a biological sense because of their implications for evolutionary fitness. In this chapter, we overview the fundamental motives framework, highlighting its applications for business decision-making in marketing, management, entrepreneurship, and finance. We then review recent research that has used this approach to study specific business-relevant topics such as risky financial decision-making, negotiation, advertising, and innovation. Bridging evolutionary biology and business, the fundamental motives framework not only provides novel insights into workplace decisions, but also holds promise as a powerful approach for understanding how behavior in business contexts connects to other aspects of human and animal behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ackerman J, Kenrick DT (2008) The costs of benefits: help-refusals highlight key trade-offs of social life. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 12(2):118–140

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackerman JM, Shapiro JR, Neuberg SL, Kenrick DT, Becker DV, Griskevicius V, Maner JK, Schaller M (2006) From out-group homogeneity to out-group heterogeneity. Psychological Science 17:836–840

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackerman JM, Kenrick DT (2009) Cooperative courtship: helping friends raise and raze relationship barriers. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 35(10):1285–1300

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackerman J, Kenrick DT, Schaller M (2007) Is friendship akin to kinship? Evol Hum Behav 28:365–374

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackerman JM, Becker DV, Mortensen CR, Sasaki T, Neuberg SL, Kenrick DT (2009) A pox on the mind: disjunction of attention and memory in the processing of physical disfigurement. J Exp Soc Psychol 45:478–485

    Google Scholar 

  • Alcock J (2005) Animal behavior: an evolutionary approach, 8th edn. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambler T, Hollier EA (2004) The waste in advertising is the part that works. J Advertising Res 44:375–389

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson RC, Reeb DM (2003) Founding family ownership and firm performance: evidence from the S&P 500. J Finance 58:1301–1326

    Google Scholar 

  • Ariely D, Loewenstein G, Prelec D (2006) Tom Sawyer and the construction of value. J Econ Behav Organ 60:1–10

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod R (1984) The evolution of cooperation. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey KG (1988) Psychological kinship: implications for the helping professions. Psychother Theor Res Pract Train 25:132–141

    Google Scholar 

  • Barclay P (2004) Trustworthiness and competitive altruism can also solve the “tragedy of the commons”. Evol Hum Behav 25:209–220

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh JA (2006) What have we been priming all these years? On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of nonconscious social behavior. Eur J Soc Psychol 36:147–168

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson M, Nettle D, Roberts G (2006) Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting. Biol Lett 2(3):412–414

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker DV, Mortensen CR, Ackerman JM, Shapiro JR, Anderson US, Sasaki T et al (2011) Self-protection and revenge-mindedness modulate detection of enemy insignia. Manuscript submitted for publication

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry LL (1983) Relationship marketing. In: Berry LL, Shostack GL, Upah GD (eds) Emerging perspectives on service marketing. American Marketing Association, Chicago, pp 25–28

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles HR, Babcock L, MacGinn KL (2005) Constraints and triggers: situational mechanics of gender in negotiation. J Pers Soc Psychol 89:951–965

    Google Scholar 

  • Burnstein E, Crandall C, Kitayama S (1994) Some neo-Darwin decision rules for altruism: weighing cues for inclusive fitness as a function of the biological importance of the decision. J Pers Soc Psychol 67:773–789

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss DM, Barnes M (1986) Preferences in human mate selection. J Pers Soc Psychol 50:559–570

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss DM, Schmitt DP (1993) Sexual strategies theory: an evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychol Rev 100:204–232

    Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini RB (2008) Influence: science and practice, 5th edn. Allyn & Bacon, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Colarelli SM (2003) No best way: an evolutionary perspective on human resource management. Praeger, Greenwich

    Google Scholar 

  • Colarelli SM, Dettman JR (2003) Intuitive evolutionary perspectives in marketing. Psychol Market 20:837–865

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides L, Tooby J (1992) Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In: Barkow JH, Cosmides L, Tooby J (eds) The adapted mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 163–228

    Google Scholar 

  • Cottrell CA, Neuberg SL (2005) Different emotional reactions to different groups: A sociofunctional threat-based approach to ‘prejudice’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88:770–789

    Google Scholar 

  • Cottrell C, Neuberg S, Li N (2007) What do people desire in others? A sociofunctional perspective on the importance of different valued characteristics. J Pers Soc Psychol 92:208–231

    Google Scholar 

  • Cross SE, Madson L (1997) Models of the self: self-construals and gender. Psychol Bull 122:5–37

    Google Scholar 

  • Curhan JR, Neale MA, Ross L, Rosencranz-Engelmann J (2008) Relational accommodation in negotiation: effects of egalitarianism and gender on economic efficiency and relational capital. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 107:192–205

    Google Scholar 

  • Daly M, Wilson M (2001) Risk-taking, intrasexual competition, and homicide. In: French JA, Kamil AC, Leger DW (eds) Evolutionary psychology and motivation, vol 7, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, pp 1–36

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1871) The descent of man, and sexual selection in relation to sex (2 vols). John Murray, London (reprinted in 1981 by Princeton University Press)

    Google Scholar 

  • DeBruine LM (2002) Facial resemblance enhances trust. Proc R Soc Lond B 269:1307–1312

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn EW, Aknin LB, Norton MI (2008) Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science 319:1687–1688

    Google Scholar 

  • Ermer E, Cosmides L, Tooby J (2007) Functional specialization and the adaptationist program. In: Gangestad SW, Simpson JA (eds) The evolution of mind: fundamental questions and controversies. The Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ermer E, Cosmides L, Tooby J (2008) Relative status regulates risky decision-making about resources in men: evidence for the co-evolution of motivation and cognition. Evol Hum Behav 29:106–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Gachter S, Kirchsteiger G (1997) Reciprocity as a contract enforcement device. Econometrica 65:833–860

    Google Scholar 

  • Fritz R (1997) Wars of succession: the blessings, curses and lessons that family owned firms offer anyone in business. Merritt Publishing, Santa Monica

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer G, Selten R (2001) Rethinking rationality. In: Gigerenzer G, Selten R (eds) Bounded rationality: the adaptive toolbox. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 1–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer G, Todd PM, ABC Research Group (1999) Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould JL, Gould CG (1989) Sexual selection. Scientific American Library; 2nd edition

    Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius V, Cialdini RB, Kenrick DT (2006a) Peacocks, Picasso, and parental investment: the effects of romantic motives on creativity. J Pers Soc Psychol 91:63–76

    Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius V, Goldstein NJ, Mortensen CR, Cialdini RB, Kenrick DT (2006b) Going along versus going alone: when fundamental motives facilitate strategic (non)conformity. J Pers Soc Psychol 91:281–294

    Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius V, Tybur JM, Sundie JM, Cialdini RB, Miller GF, Kenrick DT (2007) Blatant benevolence and conspicuous consumption: when romantic motives elicit costly displays. J Pers Soc Psychol 93:85–102

    Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius V, Goldstein NJ, Mortensen CR, Sundie JM, Cialdini RB, Kenrick DT (2009a) Fear and loving in Las Vegas: evolution, emotion, and persuasion. J Marketing Res 46:385–395

    Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius V, Tybur JM, Gangestad SW, Perea EF, Shapiro JR, Kenrick DT (2009b) Aggress to impress: hostility as an evolved context-dependent strategy. J Pers Soc Psychol 96:980–994

    Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius V, Tybur JM, Van den Bergh B (2010) Going green to be seen: status, reputation, and conspicuous conservation. J Pers Soc Psychol 98(3):392–404

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagen E (2005) Controversial issues in evolutionary psychology. In: Buss D (ed) The handbook of evolutionary psychology. John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ, pp 145–176

    Google Scholar 

  • Haley KJ, Fessler DMT (2005) Nobody’s watching? Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evol Hum Behav 26:245–256

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behavior: I and II. J Theor Biol 7:1–52

    Google Scholar 

  • Harbaugh WT, Mayr U, Burghart DR (2007) Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science 316:1622–1625

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardy CL, Van Vugt M (2006) Nice guys finish first: the competitive altruism hypothesis. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 32:1402

    Google Scholar 

  • Haselton MG, Nettle D (2006) The paranoid optimist: an integrative evolutionary model of cognitive biases. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 10:47–66

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill K, Hurtado AM (1996) Ache life history. Aldine De Gruyter, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoyer WD, MacInnis DJ (2006) Consumer behavior, 4th edn. Houghton Mifflin Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsee CK, Yang Y, Li N, Shen L (2009) Wealth, warmth and wellbeing: whether happiness is relative or absolute depends on whether it is about money, acquisition, or consumption. J Marketing Res 46(3):396–409

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen-Campbell LA, Graziano WC, West SG (1995) Dominance, prosocial orientation, and female preferences: do nice guys really finish last. J Pers Soc Psychol 68:427–440

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D, Tversky A (1979) Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47:263–291

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D, Slovic P, Tversky A (1982) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay AC, Wheeler SC, Bargh JA, Ross L (2004) Material priming: the influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal and competitive behavioral choice. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 95:83–96

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick DT, Luce CL (2000) An evolutionary life-history model of gender differences and similarities. In: Eckes T, Trautner HM (eds) The developmental social psychology of gender. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 35–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick DT, Shiota MN (2008) Approach and avoidance motivation(s): an evolutionary perspective. In: Elliot AJ (ed) Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation. Erlbaum, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick DT, Sundie JM, Kurzban R (2008) Cooperation and conflict between kith, kin, and strangers: game theory by domains. In: Crawford C, Krebs D (eds) Foundations of evolutionary psychology. Erlbaum, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick DT, Griskevicius V, Neuberg SL, Schaller M (2010) Renovating the pyramid of needs: contemporary extensions built upon ancient foundations. Perspect Psychol Sci 5:292–314

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein SB, Cosmides L, Tooby J, Chance S (2002) Decisions and the evolution of memory: multiple systems, multiple functions. Psychol Rev 109:306–329

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner JS, Keltner D (2001) Fear, anger, and risk. J Pers Soc Psychol 81:146–159

    Google Scholar 

  • Li NP, Bailey JM, Kenrick DT, Linsenmeier JA (2002) The necessities and luxuries of mate preferences: ting the trade-offs. J Pers Soc Psychol 82:947–955

    Google Scholar 

  • Maner JK, Kenrick DT, Becker DV, Robertson TE, Hofer B, Neuberg SL, Delton AW, Butner J, Schaller M (2005) Functional projection: how fundamental social motives can bias interpersonal perception. J Pers Soc Psychol 88:63–78

    Google Scholar 

  • Maner JK, Nathan DeWall C, Baumeister RF, Schaller M (2007) Does social exclusion motivate interpersonal reconnection? Resolving the “Porcupine Problem”. J Pers Soc Psychol 92:42–55

    Google Scholar 

  • McClure EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, Zarahn E, Leibenluft E, Bilder RM, Charney DS (2004) A developmental examination of gender differences in brain engagement during evaluation of threat. Biol Psychiatry 55:1047–1055

    Google Scholar 

  • McConaughy DL, Matthews CH, Fialko AS (2001) Founding family controlled firms: performance, risk and value. J Small Bus Manage 39:31–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Milinski D, Semmann D, Krambeck H (2000) Donors to charity gain in both indirect reciprocity and political reputation. Proc R Soc 269:881–883

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller GF (2000) The mating mind. Doubleday, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Møller AP, Petrie M (2002) Condition -dependence, multiple sexual signals, and immunocompetence in peacocks. Behav Ecol 13:248–253

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers D (2004) Social psychology, 8th edn. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson N (2008) Evolutionary psychology and family business: a new synthesis for theory, research, and practice. Fam Bus Rev 21:103–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson N, Björnberg A (2005) Evolutionary psychology and the family firm: structure, culture and performance. In: Tomaselli S, Melin L (eds) Family firms in the wind of change. Research Forum Proceedings, IFERA, Lausanne

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett RE, Ross L (1980) Human interferences: strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogawa S, Piller FT (2006) Reducing the risk of new product development. MIT?Sloan Manage Rev 47:65–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Öhman A, Mineka S (2001) Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol Rev 108:483–522

    Google Scholar 

  • Park JH, Schaller M (2005) Does attitude similarity serve as a heuristic cue for kinship? Evidence of an implicit cognitive association. Evol Hum Behav 26:158–170

    Google Scholar 

  • Perez-Gonzalez F (2006) Inherited control and firm performance. Am Econ Rev 96:1559–1588

    Google Scholar 

  • Pham MT (1996) Cue representation and selection effects of arousal on persuasion. J Consum Res 22(4):144–159

    Google Scholar 

  • Png I, Lehman D (2007) Managerial economics. Blackwell, Malden

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratkanis AR, Aronson E (2000) Age of propaganda: the everyday use and abuse of persuasion, 2nd edn. W. H. Freeman, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack DG, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 1:515–526

    Google Scholar 

  • Pruitt DG, Johnson DF (1970) Mediation as an aid to face saving in negotiation. J Pers Soc Psychol 14:239–246

    Google Scholar 

  • Raffia H (1982) The art and science of negotiation. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin JZ, Brown BR (1975) The social psychology of bargaining and negotiation. Academic, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Saad G (2004) Applying evolutionary psychology in understanding the representation of women in advertisements. Psychol Market 21(8):593–612

    Google Scholar 

  • Saad G (2007) The evolutionary bases of consumption. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Saad G, Gill T (2001a) The effects of a recipient’s gender in the modified dictator game. Appl Econ Lett 8:463–466

    Google Scholar 

  • Saad G, Gill T (2001b) Sex differences in the ultimatum game: an evolutionary psychology perspective. J Bioecon 3:171–193

    Google Scholar 

  • Saad G, Gill T (2003) An evolutionary psychology perspective on gift giving among young adults. Psychol Market 20:765–784

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaller M, Park JH, Kenrick DT (2007) Human evolution and social cognition. In: Dunbar RIM, Barrett L (eds) Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 491–504

    Google Scholar 

  • Schulze WS, Lubatkin MH, Dino RN, Buchholtz AK (2001) Agency relationships in family firms: theory and evidence. Organ Sci 12:99–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz N, Bless H (1991) Happy and mindless, but sad and smart? The impact of affective states on analytic reasoning. In: Forgas JP (ed) Emotion and social judgment. Pergamon, Oxford, pp 55–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Semmann D, Krambeck H, Milinski M (2005) Reputation is valuable within and outside one’s social group. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57:611–616

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherry DF, Schacter DL (1987) The evolution of multiple memory systems. Psychol Rev 94:439–454

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonton DK (1999) Origins of genius: Darwinian perspectives on creativity. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith MS, Kish BJ, Crawford CB (1987) Inheritance of wealth as human kin investment. Ethol Sociobiol 8:171–182

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon MR (2004) Consumer behavior, 6th edn. Pearson Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, New Jersey. Prentice Hall is a subsidiary of Pearson education

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonbonmatsu DM, Kardes FR (1988) The effects of physiological arousal on information processing and persuasion. J Consum Res 15:379–385

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuhlmacher AF, Walters AE (1999) Gender differences in negotiation outcome: a meta-analysis. Pers Psychol 52:653–677

    Google Scholar 

  • Sundén AE, Surette BJ (1998) Gender differences in the allocation of assets in retirement savings plans. Am Econ Rev 88:207–211

    Google Scholar 

  • Sundie JM, Cialdini RB, Griskevicius V, Kenrick DT (2006) Evolutionary social influence. In: Schaller M, Simpson JA, Kenrick DT (eds) Evolution and social psychology. Psychology Press, New York, pp 287–316

    Google Scholar 

  • The University of Tulsa College of Business Administration (2000) The Family-Owned Business Institute. [On-line] Available: http://www.galstar.com/~persson/fobi2/

  • Trivers RL (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 46:35–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers RL (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell B (ed) Sexual selection and the descent of man: 1871–1971. Aldine, Chicago, pp 136–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker B (2006) A future discounting explanation for the persistence of a mixed foraging-horticulture strategy among the Mikea of Madagascar. In: Kennett DJ, Winterhalder B (eds) Behavioral ecology and the transition to agriculture. University of California Press, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bergh B, Dewitte S (2006) Digit ratio (2D:4D) moderates the impact of sexual cues on men’s decisions in ultimatum games. Proc R Soc B-Bio 273:2091–2095

    Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bergh B, Dewitte S, Warlop L (2008) Bikinis instigate generalized impatience in intertemporal choice. J Consum Res 35:85–97

    Google Scholar 

  • Villalonga B, Amit R (2006) How do family ownership, control and management affect firm value? J Financ Econ 80:385–417

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson M, Daly M (2004) Do pretty women inspire men to discount the future? Proc R Soc B 271 Suppl 4:177–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder B (2007) Risk and decision-making. In: Dunbar R, Barrett L (eds) Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi A, Zahavi A (1997) The handicap principle: a missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford University Press, New-York, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Griskevicius, V., Ackerman, J.M., Van den Bergh, B., Li, Y.J. (2011). Fundamental Motives and Business Decisions. In: Saad, G. (eds) Evolutionary Psychology in the Business Sciences. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-92784-6_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics