Advertisement

Mother-Young and Within-Litter Relations in the European Rabbit Oryctolagus cuniculus

  • Amando Bautista
  • Margarita Martínez-Gómez
  • Robyn Hudson

The European rabbit, Oryctolagus cuniculus, in its domesticated form, is an important animal in biomedical research and in a number of countries is of economic significance for the production of meat and fur. In the wild, it is an appreciated game species, and in several countries it is also a major agricultural pest. The rabbit’s long association with humans has resulted in a large body of information on its general biology and it is now one of the most studied mammals in the wild. In whatever context, however, the rabbit is best known for its remarkable reproductive capacity. This is due in large part to the reproductive efficiency of the female (Fig. 1), a notable feature of which is the brief time mothers spend each day even with their newborn young (reviewed in Hudson and Distel 1982; 1989). After giving birth to the altricial young in a separate nursery burrow or in a chamber in the colony warren, the doe leaves, closes the entrance, and only returns to nurse for a few minutes once approximately every 24 h. As for several other lagomorphs (see below), such limited contact between mother and young is thought to have evolved to reduce the possibility of predators locating the nest from the attendance of the more conspicuous mother (Zarrow et al. 1965).

Keywords

Oryctolagus Cuniculus European Rabbit European Hare Newborn Rabbit Domestic Rabbit 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Allingham K, von Saldern C, Brennan PA, Distel H, Hudson R (1998) Endogenous expression of c-Fos in hypothalamic nuclei of neonatal rabbits coincides with their circadian pattern of suckling-associated arousal. Brain Res 783:210–218.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Altbäcker V, Hudson R, Bilkó Á (1995) Rabbit-mothers' diet influences pups' later food choice. Ethology 99:107–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bautista A, Drummond H, Martínez-Gómez M, Hudson R (2003) Thermal benefit of sibling presence in the newborn rabbit. Dev Psychobiol 43:208–215.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bautista A (2005) Conducta de agrupamiento en conejos neonatos y su relación con temperatura corporal, ingestión de leche y crecimiento individual. Tesis de Doctorado. Universidad Veracruzana, Mexico.Google Scholar
  5. Bautista A, Mendoza-Degante M, Coureaud G, Martínez-Gómez M, Hudson R (2005) Scramble competition in newborn domestic rabbits for an unusually restricted milk supply. Anim Behav 70:1011–1021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bilkó Á, Altbäcker V, Hudson R (1994) Transmission of food preference in the rabbit: the means of information transfer. Physiol Behav 56:907–912.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Broekhuizen S, Maarskamp F (1980) Behaviour of does and leverets of the European hare (Lepus europaeus) whilst nursing. J Zool Lond 191:487–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Broekhuizen S, Mulder JL (1983) Differences and similarities in nursing behaviour of hares and rabbits. Acta Zool Fen 174:61–63.Google Scholar
  9. Broekhuizen S, Bouman E, Went W (1986) Variation in timing of nursing in the Brown Hare (Lepus europaeus) and the European Rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Mammal Rev 16:139–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Caba M, Rovirosa MJ, Silver R (2003) Suckling and genital stroking induces Fos expression in hypothalamic oxytocinergic neurons of rabbit pups. Dev Brain Res 143:119–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Caldelas I, Chimal-Monroy J, Martínez-Gómez M, Hudson R (2005) Non-photic circadian entrainment in mammals: a brief review and proposal for study during development. Biol Rhythm Res 36:23–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Coureaud G, Schaal B, Coudert P, Rideaud P, Fortun-Lamothe L, Hudson R, Orgeur P (2000) Immediate postnatal suckling in the rabbit: its influence on pup survival and growth. Rep Nutricion et Development 40:19–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Coureaud G, Schaal B, Hudson R, Orgeur P, Coudert P (2002) Transnatal olfactory continuity in the rabbit: behavioral evidence and short-term consequences of its disruption. Dev Psychobiol 40:372–390.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cowan DP, Bell DJ (1986) Leporid social behaviour and social organization. Mammal Rev 16:169–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Distel H, Hudson R (1984) Nipple-search performance by rabbit pups: changes with age and time of day. Anim Behav 32:501–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Distel H, Hudson R (1985) The contribution of the olfactory and tactile modalities to the nipple-search behaviour of newborn rabbits. J Comp Physiol A 157:599–605.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Drummond H, Vázquez E, Sánchez-Colón S, Martínez-Gómez M, Hudson R (2000) Competition for milk in the domestic rabbit: survivors benefit from littermate deaths. Ethology 106:511–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dyballa P (1978) Schneehäsin beim Säugen. Wild und Hund 81:265.Google Scholar
  19. Ecke DH (1955) The reproductive cycle of the Mearns cottontail in Illinois. Am Midland Natural 53:294–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Escobar C, Hudson R, Martínez-Gómez M, Aguilar-Roblero R (2000) Metabolic correlates of the circadian pattern of suckling-associated arousal in young rabbits. J Comp Physiol A 186:33–38.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fuchs AR, Dawood MY (1980) Oxytocin release and uterine activation during parturition in rabbits. Endocrinology 107:1117–1126.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fuchs AR, Cubile L, Dawood MY, Jørgensen FS (1984) Release of oxytocin and prolactin by suckling in rabbits throughout lactation. Endocrinology 114:462–469.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. González-Mariscal G, Chirino R, Hudson R (1994) Prolactin stimulates emission of nipple pheromone in ovariectomized New Zealand white rabbits. Biol Rep 50:373–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. González-Mariscal G, Rosenblatt JS (1996) Maternal behavior in rabbits. A historical and multidisciplinary perspective. In: Rosenblatt JS, Snowdon CT (eds) Advances in the study of behavior, vol 25. Parental care: evolution, mechanisms and adaptive significance. Academic Press, New York, pp 333–360.Google Scholar
  25. Hudson R (1985) Do newborn rabbits learn the odor stimuli releasing nipple-search behavior? Dev Psychobiol 18:575–585.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hudson R (1998) Potential of the newborn rabbit for circadian rhythms research. Biol Rhythm Res 29:546–555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hudson R, Distel H (1982) The pattern of behaviour of rabbit pups in the nest. Behaviour 79:255–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hudson R, Distel H (1983) Nipple location by newborn rabbits: behavioural evidence for pheromonal guidance. Behaviour 85:260–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hudson R, Distel H (1984) Nipple-search pheromone in rabbits: dependence on season and reproductive state. J Comp Physiol A 155:13–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hudson R, Distel H (1989) Temporal pattern of suckling in rabbit pups: a model of circadian synchrony between mother and young. In: Reppert SM (ed) Development of circadian rhythmicity and photoperiodism in mammals. Perinatology Press, Boston, pp 83–102.Google Scholar
  31. Hudson R, Distel H (1990) Sensitivity of female rabbits to changes in photoperiod as measured by pheromone emission. J Comp Physiol A 167:225–230.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hudson R, González-Mariscal G, Beyer C (1990) Chin marking behavior, sexual receptivity, and pheromone emission in steroid-treated, ovariectomized rabbits. Hormones Behav 24:1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hudson R, Müller A, Kennedy GA (1995a) Parturition in the rabbit is compromised by daytime nursing: the role of oxytocin. Biol Rep 53:519–524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hudson R, Distel H (1995b) On the nature and action of the rabbit nipple-search pheromone: a review. In: Apfelbach R, Müller-Schwarze D, Reuter K, Weiler E (eds) Chemical signals in vertebrates VII. Elsevier Science, London, pp 223–232.Google Scholar
  35. Hudson R, Bilkó Á, Altbäcker V (1996) Nursing, weaning and the development of independent feeding in the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus).Z Säugetierkde 61:39–48.Google Scholar
  36. Hudson R, Cruz Y, Lucio RL, Ninomiya J, Martínez-Gómez M (1999a) Temporal and behavioral patterning of parturition in rabbits and rats. Physiol Behav 66:599–604.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hudson R, Schaal B, Bilkó Á (1999b) Transmission of olfactory information from mother to young in the European rabbit. In: Box HO, Gibson KR (eds) Mammalian social learning: comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 141–157.Google Scholar
  38. Jilge B (1993) The ontogeny of circadian rhythms in the rabbit. J Biol Rhythms 8:247–260.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Jilge B (1995) Ontogeny of the rabbit's circadian rhythms without an external zeitgeber. Physiol Behav 58:131–140.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Jilge B, Kuhnt B, Landerer W, Rest S (2000) Circadian thermoregulation in suckling rabbit pups. J Biol Rhythms 15:329–335.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Jilge B, Kuhnt B, Landerer W, Rest S (2001) Circadian thermoregulation in suckling rabbit pups and their does. Lab Anim Sci 35:1–10.Google Scholar
  42. Jilge B, Hudson R (2001) Diversity and development of circadian rhythms in the European rabbit. Chronobiology Int 18:1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Keil W, von Stralendorff F, Hudson R (1990) A behavioral bioassay for analysis of rabbit nipple-search pheromone. Physiol Behav 47:525–529.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Lincoln DW (1974) Suckling: a time constant in the nursing behaviour of the rabbit. Physiol Behav 13:711–714.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Martínez-Gómez M, Lucio RA, Carro M, Pacheco P, Hudson R (1997) Striated muscles and scent glands associated with the vaginal tract of the rabbit. Anatom Rec 247:486–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Martínez-Gómez M, Juárez M, Distel H, Hudson R (2004) Overlapping litters and reproductive performance in the domestic rabbit. Physiol Behav 82:629–636.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Mock WD, Parker AG (1997) The evolution of sibling rivalry. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  48. Moncomble RS, Coureaud G, Quennedey B, Langlois D, Perrier G, Schaal B (2005) The mammary pheromone of the rabbit: from where does it come? Anim Behav 69:29–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Ninomiya-Alarcón JG, Hudson R, Reyes-Guerrero G, Barrera-Mera B, Guevara-Guzmán R (2004) Effect of photoperiod on the mechanical response of the pregnant rabbit uterus to oxytocin. Am J Physiol: Regulative, Integrative, Comp Physiol 287:R174–R180.Google Scholar
  50. Pacheco-Cobos L, Rosetti M, Distel H, Hudson R (2003) To stay or not to stay: the contribution of tactile and thermal cues to coming to rest in newborn rabbits. J Comp Physiol A 189:383–389.Google Scholar
  51. Patton NM (1994) Colony husbandry. In: Manning PJ, Ringler DH, Newcomer CE (eds) The biology of the laboratory rabbit. Academic Press, New York, pp 27–45.Google Scholar
  52. Pongrácz P, Altbäcker V (1999) The effect of early handling is dependent upon the state of the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) pups around nursing. Dev Psychobiol 35:241–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Pongrácz P, Altbäcker V (2003) Arousal, but not nursing, is necessary to elicit a decreased fear reaction towards humans in rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) pups. Dev Psychobiol 43:192–199.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Rongstad OJ, Tester JR (1971) Behavior and maternal relations of young snowshoe hares. J Wildl Manage 35:338–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Ross S, Denenberg VH, Frommer GP, Sawin PB (1959) Genetic, physiological and behavioral background of reproduction in the rabbit. V. Nonretrieving of neonates. J Mammal 40:91–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Schaal B, Coureaud G, Langlois D, Ginies C, Sémon E, Perrier G (2003) The mammary pheromone of the rabbit: chemical and behavioural characterisation. Nature 424:68–72.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Schley P (1981) Geruchssinn und Saugverhalten bei Jungkaninchen. Kleintierpraxis 26:197–264.Google Scholar
  58. Semke E, Distel H, Hudson R (1995) Specific enhancement of olfactory receptor sensitivity associated with foetal learning of food odors in the rabbit. Naturwissenschaften 82:148–149.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Sorensen MF, Rogers JP, Baskett TS (1972) Parental behavior in swamp rabbits. J Mammal 53:840–849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Sulloway JF (2001) Birth order, sibling competition, and human behavior. In: Harmon RH (ed) Conceptual challenges in evolutionary psychology: innovative research strategies. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, pp 39–83.Google Scholar
  61. Zarrow MX, Denenberg VH, Anderson CO (1965) Rabbit: frequency of suckling in the pup. Science 150:1835–1836.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Amando Bautista
    • 1
  • Margarita Martínez-Gómez
    • 1
    • 2
  • Robyn Hudson
    • 2
  1. 1.Centro Tlaxcala de Biología de la ConductaUNAM-UATTlaxcalaMexico
  2. 2.Instituto de Investigaciones BiomédicasUniversidad Nacional Autónoma de MéxicoMexico D.FMexico

Personalised recommendations