Advertisement

Emotionen pp 262-271 | Cite as

Linguistische Perspektiven auf Emotion und Sprache

Chapter

Zusammenfassung

Emotionen sind konstitutive Phänomene des menschlichen Lebens und Erlebens (vgl. Schwarz-Friesel 2013). Dementsprechend werden sie in vielfältiger Weise durch Sprache ausgedrückt. Das Sprechen über Gefühle und andere innere Zustände wie Wünsche, Absichten und Gedanken ist ein wesentlicher Bestandteil menschlicher Kommunikation. »It is unavoidable that emotions color language, as language is part of our everyday life« (Foolen 2015, 244).

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Literatur

  1. Al-Mosaiwi, Mohammed/Johnstone, Tom: In an absolute state. Elevated use of absolutist words is a marker specific to anxiety, depression, and suicidal ideation. In: Clinical Psychological Science 45 (2018), 1–14.Google Scholar
  2. Bahn, Daniela/Kauschke, Christina/Vesker, Michael/ Schwarzer, Gudrun: Perception of valence and arousal in German emotion terms. A comparison between 9-yearold children and adults. In: Applied Psycholinguistics (2017a), 1–19.Google Scholar
  3. Bahn, Daniela/Vesker, Michael/García Alanis, José C./ Schwarzer, Gudrun/Kauschke, Christina: Age-dependent positivity-bias in children’s processing of emotion terms. In: Frontiers in Psychology 8 (2017b), 1268.Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, Simon/Golan, Ofer/Wheelwright, Sally/Granader, Yael/Hill, Jacqueline: Emotion word comprehension from 4 to 16 years old. A developmental survey. In: Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience 2 (2010), 109.Google Scholar
  5. Barth, Alfred/Küfferle, Bernd: Die Entwicklung eines Sprichworttests zur Erfassung konkretistischer Denkstörungen bei schizophrenen Patienten. In: Der Nervenarzt 72/11 (2001), 853–858.Google Scholar
  6. Bayer, Mareike/Schacht, Annekathrin: Event-related brain responses to emotional words, pictures, and faces – A cross-domain comparison. In: Frontiers in Psychology 5 (2014), 1106.Google Scholar
  7. Beck, Luna/Kumschick, Irina R./Eid, Michael/Klann-Delius, Gisela: Relationship between language competence and emotional competence in middle childhood. In: Emotion 12/3 (2012), 503–514.Google Scholar
  8. Bishop, Dorothy V. M./Snowling, Margaret J./Thompson, Paul A./Greenhalgh, Trisha: Phase 2 of CATALISE. A multinational and multidisciplinary delphi consensus study of problems with language development. Terminology. In: Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 58/10 (2017), 1068–1080.Google Scholar
  9. Bretherton, Inge/Beeghly, Marjorie: Talking about internal states. The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. In: Developmental Psychology 18/6 (1982), 906–921.Google Scholar
  10. Bühler, Karl: Sprachtheorie. Die Darstellungsfunktion der Sprache [1934]. Stuttgart 1999.Google Scholar
  11. Citron, Francesca M. M./Goldberg, Adele E.: Metaphorical sentences are more emotionally engaging than their literal counterparts. In: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26/11 (2014), 2585–2595.Google Scholar
  12. Conti-Ramsden, Gina/Botting, Nicola: Social difficulties and victimization in children with SLI at 11 years of age. In: Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 47/1 (2004), 145–161.Google Scholar
  13. d’Avis, Franz: Exklamativsatz. In: Jörg Meibauer/Markus Steinbach/Hans Altmann (Hg.): Satztypen des Deutschen. Berlin/Boston (MA) 2013, 171–201.Google Scholar
  14. Dijksterhuis, Ap/Aarts, Henk: On wildebeests and humans. The preferential detection of negative stimuli. In: Psychological Science 14/1 (2003), 14–18.Google Scholar
  15. Eichstaedt, Johannes C./Smith, Robert J./Merchant, Raina M./Ungar, Lyle H./Crutchley, Patrick/Preotiuc-Pietro, Daniel/Asch, David A./Schwartz, H. Andrew: Facebook language predicts depression in medical records. In: PNAS 115/44 (2018), 11203–11208.Google Scholar
  16. Foolen, Ad: Word valence and its effects. In: Ulrike M. Lüdtke (Hg.): Emotion in Language. Theory – Research – Application. Philadelphia 2015, 241–256.Google Scholar
  17. Foolen, Ad: The relevance of emotion for language and linguistics. In: Ad Foolen/Ulrike M. Lüdtke/Timothy P. Racine/Jordan Zlatev (Hg.): Moving Ourselves, Moving Others. Amsterdam 2012, 347–368.Google Scholar
  18. Hofmann, Markus J./Kuchinke, Lars/Tamm, Sascha/Võ, Melissa L. H./Jacobs, Arthur M.: Affective processing within 1/10th of a second: High arousal is necessary for early facilitative processing of negative but not positive words. In: Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 9/4 (2009), 389–397.Google Scholar
  19. Izard, Carroll/Fine, Sarah/Schultz, David/Mostow, Allison/ Ackerman, Brian/Youngstrom, Eric: Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. In: Psychological Science 12/1 (2001), 18–23.Google Scholar
  20. Jacobs, Arthur M./Võ, Melissa L.-H./Briesemeister, Benny B./Conrad, Markus/Hofmann, Markus J./Kuchinke, Lars/ Lüdtke, Jana/Braun, Mario: 10 years of BAWLing into affective and aesthetic processes in reading. What are the echoes? In: Frontiers in Psychology 6 (2015), 714.Google Scholar
  21. Kauschke, Christina: Sprechen über Inneres – die Versprachlichung von Emotionen im Kindesalter. In: SALBulletin 145 (2012), 4–16.Google Scholar
  22. Kauschke, Christina/Bahn, Daniela/Vesker, Michael/Schwarzer, Gudrun: Die semantische Repräsentation von Emotionsbegriffen bei Kindern im Grundschulalter. In: Kindheit und Entwicklung 26/4 (2017), 251–260.Google Scholar
  23. Kauschke, Christina/Bahn, Daniela/Vesker, Michael/ Schwarzer, Gudrun: Review: The role of emotional valence for the processing of facial and verbal stimuli – positivity or negativity bias? In: Frontiers in Psychology 10 (2019), article 1654.Google Scholar
  24. Kauschke, Christina/Kircher, Tilo/Mueller, Nadine/Nagels, Arne: Do patients with depression prefer literal or metaphorical expressions for internal states? Evidence from sentence completion and elicited production. In: Frontiers in Psychology 9 (2018), article 1326.Google Scholar
  25. Kauschke, Christina/Klann-Delius, Gisela: The acquisition of verbal expressions for internal states in German. A descriptive, explorative, longitudinal study. In: Susanne Niemeier/ René Dirven (Hg.): The Language of Emotions. Conceptualization, expression, and theoretical foundation. Amsterdam/Philadelphia 1997, 173–194.Google Scholar
  26. Kauschke, Christina/van der Beek, Bettina/Kamp-Becker, Inge: Narratives of Girls and Boys with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Gender Differences in Narrative Competence and Internal State Language. In: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 46/3 (2015), 840–852.Google Scholar
  27. Klann-Delius, Gisela: Emotion in language. In: Ulrike M. Lüdtke (Hg.): Emotion in Language. Theory – Research – Application. Philadelphia 2015, 135–156.Google Scholar
  28. Kousta, Stavroula-Thaleia/Vinson, David P./Vigliocco, Gabriella: Emotion words, regardless of polarity, have a processing advantage over neutral words. In: Cognition 112/3 (2009), 473–481.Google Scholar
  29. Kövecses, Zoltán: Metaphor. A practical introduction [2002]. New York 2010.Google Scholar
  30. Kristen, Susanne/Sodian, Beate/Licata, Maria/Thoermer, Claudia/Poulin-Dubois, Diane: The development of internal state language during the third year of life. A longitudinal parent report study. In: Infant and Child Development 21/6 (2012), 634–645.Google Scholar
  31. Lakoff, George/Johnson, Mark: Metaphors we live by [1980]. Chicago 2002.Google Scholar
  32. Levy, Judith/Kauschke, Christina: Affective and internal state language in high-functioning autism. In: Ulrike M. Lüdtke (Hg.): Emotion in Language. Theory – Research – Application. Philadelphia 2015, 421–444.Google Scholar
  33. Lindsay, Geoff/Dockrell, Julie E.: Longitudinal patterns of behavioral, emotional, and social difficulties and self-concepts in adolescents with a history of specific language impairment. In: Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 43/4 (2012), 445–460.Google Scholar
  34. Losh, Molly/Capps, Lisa: Narrative ability in high-functioning children with autism or asperger’s syndrome. In: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 33/3 (2003), 239–251.Google Scholar
  35. Müller, Cornelia: Waking metaphors. Embodied cognition in multimodal discourse. In: Beate Hampe (Hg.): Metaphor. From Embodied Cognition to Discourse. Cambridge 2017, 297–316.Google Scholar
  36. Müller, Cornelia/Kappelhoff, Hermann: Hand gestures as expressive movements (Ch. 9.1); Interaffectivity and the emergence of metaphor in a group discussion (Ch. 9.2). In: Cornelia Müller/Hermann Kappelhoff: Cinematic Metaphor. Experience – Affectivity – Temporality. Berlin/ Boston 2018, 146–154.Google Scholar
  37. Nelson, Katherine: Language in cognitive development. Emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge 1996.Google Scholar
  38. Noterdaeme, Michele: Autismus-Spektrum-Störungen. Ein Überblick zum aktuellen Forschungsstand. In: Klinische Pädiatrie 223 Suppl 1 (2011), E1–E15.Google Scholar
  39. Pérez-Edgar, Koraly/Fox, Nathan A.: Temperamental contributions to children’s performance in an emotion-word processing task. A behavioral and electrophysiological study. In: Brain and Cognition 65/1 (2007), 22–35.Google Scholar
  40. Reed, Andrew E./Carstensen, Laura L.: The theory behind the age-related positivity effect. In: Frontiers in Psychology 3 (2012), 339.Google Scholar
  41. Rude, Stephanie/Gortner, Eva-Maria/Pennebaker, James: Language use of depressed and depression-vulnerable college students. In: Cognition & Emotion 18/8 (2004), 1121–1133.Google Scholar
  42. Rumpf, Anna-Lena/Kamp-Becker, Inge/Becker, Katja/ Kauschke, Christina: Narrative competence and internal state language of children with asperger syndrome and ADHD. In: Research in Developmental Disabilities 33/5 (2012), 1395–1407.Google Scholar
  43. Schwarz-Friesel, Monika: Language and emotion. The cognitive linguistic perspective. In: Ulrike M. Lüdtke (Hg.): Emotion in Language. Theory – Research – Application. Philadelphia 2015, 157–173.Google Scholar
  44. Schwarz-Friesel, Monika: Sprache und Emotion. [2007]. Stuttgart, Tübingen 2013.Google Scholar
  45. Siller, Michael/Swanson, Meghan R./Serlin, Gayle/George, Ann: Internal state language in the storybook narratives of children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Investigating relations to theory of mind abilities. In: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8/5 (2014), 589–596.Google Scholar
  46. Sylvester, Teresa/Braun, Mario/Schmidtke, David/Jacobs, Arthur M.: The berlin affective word list for children (kid- BAWL). Exploring processing of affective lexical semantics in the visual and auditory modalities. In: Frontiers in Psychology 7 (2016), 969.Google Scholar
  47. Vaish, Amrisha/Grossmann, Tobias/Woodward, Amanda: Not all emotions are created equal. The negativity bias in social-emotional development. In: Psychological Bulletin 134/3 (2008), 383–403.Google Scholar
  48. Vigliocco, Gabriella/Kousta, Stavroula-Thaleia/Della Rosa, Pasquale A./Vinson, David P./Tettamanti, Marco/Devlin, Joseph T./Cappa, Stefano F.: The neural representation of abstract words. The role of emotion. In: Cerebral Cortex 24/7 (2013), 1767–1777.Google Scholar
  49. Võ, Melissa L.-H./Conrad, Markus/Kuchinke, Lars/Urton, Karolina/Hofmann, Markus J./Jacobs, Arthur M.: The berlin affective word list reloaded (BAWL-R). In: Behavior Research Methods 41/2 (2009), 534–538.Google Scholar
  50. Widen, Sherri C./Russell, James A.: Children acquire emotion categories gradually. In: Cognitive Development 23/2 (2008), 291–312.Google Scholar
  51. Zeelenberg, René/Wagenmakers, Eric-Jan/Rotteveel, Mark: The impact of emotion on perception: Bias or enhanced processing? In: Psychological Science 17/4 (2006), 287– 291.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Deutschland, ein Teil von Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.MarburgDeutschland

Personalised recommendations