Skip to main content

Zur Wahl des repräsentierten Raumausschnitts beim Aufbau mentaler Modelle während der Textrezeption

  • Chapter
Perspektive in Sprache und Raum

Zusammenfassung

Nach der Theorie mentaler Modelle (z.B. Garnham, 1987; Glenberg & Langston, 1992; Johnson-Laird, 1983) wird beim Textverstehen zusätzlich zu Repräsentationen der sprachlichen Oberfläche und des propositionalen Textgehalts auch eine Repräsentation des im Text geschilderten Sachverhalts aufgebaut (“mentales Modell”). Das Bilden eines mentalen Modells macht nach Johnson-Laird Textverstehen aus (Johnson-Laird, 1983, S. 415ff). Zur Begründung, warum zusätzlich zu der Ebene propositionaler Textrepräsentationen eine Ebene der Sachverhaltsrepräsentationen postuliert wird, verweist Johnson-Laird (1983) vor allem auf zwei Funktionen (vgl. auch Johnson-Laird, 1989): (1) Die Sachverhaltsrepräsentation, die zu einem Textsegment gebildet wird, liefert den Kontext für die Interpretation des nachfolgenden Textsegments, insbesondere für die Referenzauflösung, indem sie stellvertretende “Tokens” für die Entitäten der Diskurswelt bereithält, und (2) Sachverhaltsrepräsentationen ermöglichen es, sprachliche Information mit nichtsprachlich erworbenem Wissen bzw. aktuell Wahrgenommenem in Beziehung zu setzen oder zu integrieren, da sie im Prinzip von derselben Art sind wie Repräsentationen, die bei nichtsprachlichen kognitiven Tätigkeiten aufgebaut werden. Es gibt demnach eine Repräsentationsebene, die als gemeinsame Projektionsfläche für sprachliche und nichtsprachliche Information fungiert: Die Repräsentationen auf dieser Ebene “... relate words to the world by way of conception and perception” (Johnson-Laird, 1983, S. 397).

Die Untersuchung wurde im Rahmen des von der Deutschen Forschungsgemeinschaft unterstützten Forschungsprojekts “Nichtsprachliche Kognition und Textverstehen” durchgeführt. Wir danken Volker Bleckmann und Martin Kindsmüller für die Hilfe bei der Erhebung und Auswertung der Daten und Mark Erichsen für die Programmierung der Versuchssteuerung.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 49.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Literatur

  • Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, J. B., Turner, T. J. & Bower, G. H. (1979). Point of view in narrative comprehension, memory, and production. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 187–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P. (1990). The causal inference marker: Towards a process model of inference generation in text comprehension. In D. Balota, G. Flores D’Arcais & K. Rayner (Eds.), Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 423–445). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britton, B. K., van Dusen, L., Glynn, S. M., & Hemphill, D. (1990). The impact of inferences on instructional text. In A. C. Graesser & G. H. Bower (Eds.), Inferences and text comprehension (pp. 53–70). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. R. (1967). The suppression of visualization by reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 19, 289–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. R. (1970). An extension of the conflict between visualization and reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 91–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denis, M. & Zimmer, H. D. (1992). Analog properties of cognitive maps constructed from verbal descriptions. Psychological Research, 54, 286–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eddy, J. K. & Glass, A. L. (1981). Reading and listening to high and low imagery sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 296–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich, K. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1982). Spatial descriptions and referential continuity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 296–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finke, R. A. (1989). Principles of mental imagery. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, C. R. & Bloom, P. R. (1988). Causal reasoning in the comprehension of simple narrative texts. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 235–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foertsch, J. & Gernsbacher, M. A. (1994). In search of complete comprehension: getting “minimalists” to work. Discourse Processes, 18, 271–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, N., Tversky, B. & Coon, V. (1992). Switching points of view in spatial mental models. Memory & Cognition, 20, 507–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garnham, A. (1981). Anaphoric references to instances, instantiated and non- instantiated categories: A reading time study. British Journal of Psychology, 72, 377–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garnham, A. (1987). Mental models as representations of discourse and text. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A. (1990). Language comprehension as structure building. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., Varner, K. R. & Faust, M. (1990). Investigating differences in general comprehension skill. Journal o f Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 430–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilhooly, K.J., Keane, R. H., Logie, R. H. & Erdos, G. (Eds.).(1990). Lines of thinking. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glass, A. L., Milien, D. R., Beck, L. G. & Eddy, J. K. (1985). Representation of images in sentence verification. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 442–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M. & Langston, W E. (1992). Comprehension of illustrated text: Pictures help to build mental models. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 129–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Meyer, M. & Lindem, K. (1987). Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 69–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., Singer, M. & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Habel, Ch., Kaup, B. & Kelter, S. (im Druck). Embodied representations are part of a grouping of representations (Commentary on the target article “What memory is for” by A. M. Glenberg), Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Habel, Ch. & Kelter, S. (1994). Die Funktion nichtsprachlicher Repräsentationen, insbesondere räumlicher und visueller Vorstellungen, für das Textverstehen. Sachbeihilfe-Antrag an die DFG.

    Google Scholar 

  • Habel, C. (1986). Prinzipien der Referenzialität. Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heim, I. (1982). The semantics of definite and indefinite noun phrases. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Amherst (distributed by SFB 99, Univ. Konstanz).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hron, A., Kurbjuhn, L, Mandl, H. & Schnotz, W. (1985). Structural inferences in reading and listening. In G. Rickheit & H. Strohner (Eds.), Inferences in text processing (pp. 221–245). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jakimik, J. & Glenberg, A. M. (1990). Verbal learning meets psycholinguistics: Modality effects in the comprehension of anaphora. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 582–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, R N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1989). Mental models. In M. I. Posner (Ed.) Foundations of cognitive science (pp. 469–499). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1996). Images, models and propositional representations. In M. de Vega, M.J. Intons-Peterson, P.N. Johnson-Laird, M. Denis & M. Marschark (Eds.), Models of visuospatial cognition (pp. 90–127). New York / Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. & Byrne, R. M. J. (1991). Deduction. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamp, H. (1981). A theory of truth and semantic representation. In J. Groenendijk, T. Janssen & M. Stokhof (Eds.), Formal methods in the study of language: pt 1 (pp. 277–322). Amsterdam: Mathematish Centrum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelter, S., Kaup, B. & Habel, Ch. (im Druck). Taking the functional aspects of mental models as a starting point for studying discourse comprehension. In Ch. Habel & G. Rickheit (Eds.), Mental Models in Discourse Processing and Reasoning. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, N. H. (1987). Locational representation in imagery: The third dimension. Memory & Cognition, 15, 521–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, N. H. (1993). Rate of imagery processing in two versus three dimensions. Memory & Cognition, 21, 467–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1975). Information representation in visual images. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 341–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. & Schin, L. M. (1994). Visual mental images in the brain: current issues. In M. J. Farah & G. Ratcliff (Eds.), The neuropsychology of high- level vision (pp. 269–296). Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuospatial working memory. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H. & Machetti, C. (1991).. Visuo-spatial working memory: Visual, spatial or central executive? In R. H. Logie & M. Denis (Eds.), Mental images in human cognition (pp. 77–102). Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mani, K. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1982). The mental representation of spatial descriptions. Memory & Cognition, 10, 181–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKoon, G., Wald, G., Ratcliff, R. & Sproat, R. (1993). Morphosyntactic and pragmatic factors affecting the accessibility of discourse entities. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 56–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, D. G., Bower, G. H. & Greenspan, S. L. (1989). Updating situation models during narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 292–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, D. G., Bower, G. H. & Greenspan, S. L. (1990). Situation-based inferences during narrative comprehension. In A. C. Graesser & G. H. Bower (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 25. Inferences and text comprehension (pp. 123–135). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, D. G., Greenspan, S. L. & Bower, G. H. (1987). Accessibility and situation models in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 165–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müsseier, J. & Rickheit, G. (1990). The cognitive resolution of anaphoric noun references. Journal of Semantics, 7, 221–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müsseier, J., Rickheit, G. & Strohner, H. (1985). Influences of modality, text difficulty, and processing control on inferences in text processing. In G. Rickheit & H. Strohner (Eds.), Inferences in text processing (pp. 247–271). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R. S. (1981). Speech, understanding, and reading: Some differences and similarities. In O. V. L. Tzeng & H. Singer (Eds.), Perception of print: Reading research in experimental psychology (pp. 257–289). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, E. J. & Albrecht, J. E. (1992). Comprehension strategies in the development of a mental model. Journal o f Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 777–784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, S. E. (1978). Fundamental aspects of cognitive representation. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (eds.), Cognition and categorization (S.259–303). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrig, W. & Kintsch, W. (1985). Propositional and situational representations of text. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 503–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, M. J., Thomas, J. E. & Johnson, H. (1977). Imagery, rehearsal, and the compatibility of input-output tasks. Memory & Cognition, 5, 415–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, G. (1991). Encoding and maintenance of information in visual working memory. In R. H. Logie & M. Denis (Eds.), Mental images in human cognition (S.95–104). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Radvansky, G. A., Spieler, D. H. & Zacks, R. T. (1993). Mental model organization. Journal o f Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 95–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinck, M. & Bower, G. H. (1995). Anaphora resolution and the focus of attention in situation models. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 110–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanford, A. J. & Garrod, S. C. (1981). Understanding written language. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smyth, M. M. & Scholey, K. A. (1994). Interference in immediate spatial memory. Memory & Cognition, 22, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk, T. A. & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Vega, M. (1995). Backward updating of mental models during continuous reading of narratives. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 373–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagener-Wender, M. & Wender, K. F. (1990). Expectations, mental representations, and spatial inferences. In A. C. Graesser & G. H. Bower (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 25. Inferences and text comprehension (pp. 137–157). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, S. G., Rinck, M., McNamara, T. P., Bower, G. H. & Morrow, D. G. (1993). Mental models and narrative comprehension: some qualification. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 141–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, H. D. (1993). Modalitätsspezifische Systeme der Repräsentation und Verarbeitung von Information. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 201, 203–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A. & Graesser, A. C. (1993). Reading goals and situation models, Psycoloquy, 4 (3) reading-inference.4.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Carla Umbach Michael Grabski Robin Hörnig

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1997 Deutscher Universitäts-Verlag GmbH, Wiesbaden

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kaup, B., Kelter, S., Habel, C., Clauser, C. (1997). Zur Wahl des repräsentierten Raumausschnitts beim Aufbau mentaler Modelle während der Textrezeption. In: Umbach, C., Grabski, M., Hörnig, R. (eds) Perspektive in Sprache und Raum. Studien zur Kognitionswissenschaft. Deutscher Universitätsverlag. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-322-95359-9_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-322-95359-9_3

  • Publisher Name: Deutscher Universitätsverlag

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-8244-4231-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-322-95359-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics