Skip to main content

Understanding the Formation of Human Habits: An Analysis of Mechanisms of Habitual Behaviour

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Psychology of Habit

Abstract

In this chapter, we address the mechanisms of habit to promote a better understanding and examination of how habits are learned and maintained in humans. Because habits are often conceived of as automated behaviours resulting from learning and practice, we start with the typical features that represent automatic processes. Next, we discuss how habitual mechanisms might evolve in human action, starting from automatic stimulus response behaviour to automatic execution of routines or habitual skills that are conditional of the presence of desired goals. We particularly address how the preparation, selection and execution of habitually performed behaviour (i.e. the well-trained sensorimotor processes that render human interaction with the environment possible) relies on the capacity to represent actions in terms of their effects, and how activating these effect representations controls the associated behaviour. Then, we discuss the mechanisms of habit from a motivational perspective to understand how motivation plays a role in in habitual behaviour. We conclude this chapter with a set of future directions that may challenge the understanding of the mechanisms of habits.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aarts, H., & Custers, R. (2009). Habit, action and consciousness (2009). In W. P. Banks (Ed.), Encyclopaedia of consciousness (Vol. 1, pp. 315–328). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Wegner, D. M. (2005). On the inference of personal authorship: Enhancing experienced agency by priming effect information. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 439–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 53–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2003). The silence of the library: Environment, situational norm, and social behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aarts, H., Verplanken, B., & van Knippenberg, A. (1998). Predicting behavior from actions in the past: Repeated decision making or a matter of habit? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1355–1374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ach, N. (1910). Ãœber den Willensakt und das Temperament. Leipzig: Quelle & Meyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, J. A. (1971). A closed-loop theory of motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 3, 111–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. (2004). An integrated theory of the mind. Psychological Review, 111, 1036–1060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, efficiency, intention, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. C. (2001). Reward learning: Reinforcement, incentives, and expectations. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 40, pp. 223–278). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2009). The unconscious eye opener pupil dilation reveals strategic recruitment of resources upon presentation of subliminal reward cues. Psychological Science, 20, 1313–1315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. L., & Carr, T. H. (1989). Automaticity in skill acquisition: Mechanisms for reducing interference in concurrent performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 686–700.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleeremans, A., Destrebecqz, A., & Boyer, M. (1998). Implicit learning: News from the front. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 406–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colwill, R. M., & Rescorla, R. A. (1985). Postconditioning devaluation of a reinforcer affects instrumental responding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 120–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2005). Positive affect as implicit motivator: On the nonconscious operation of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 129–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science, 329, 47–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danner, U. N., Aarts, H., & de Vries, N. K. (2008). Habit and intention in the prediction of behaviors: The role of frequency, stability and accessibility of past behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 245–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, D. (1980). How is weakness of the will possible? In D. Davidson (Ed.), Essays on actions and events (pp. 21–42). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Graaf, B. A. (2012). Gevaarlijke vrouwen. Tien militante vrouwen in het vizier. Amsterdam: Boom.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., Tanaka, A., Moors, A., & Tibboel, H. (2018). Kicking the habit: Why evidence for habits in humans might be overestimated. Motivation Science, 4, 50–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, A., & Balleine, B. (1994). Motivational control of goal-directed action. Animal Learning & Behavior, 22, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, A., & Balleine, B. (1995). Motivational control of instrumental action. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 162-167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science, 298, 1191–1194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fourneret, P., & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1133–1140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. J., & Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, 355, 1771–1788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, B., Abraham, C., Lally, P., & de Bruijn, G.-J. (2012). Towards parsimony in habit measurement: Testing the convergent and predictive validity of an automaticy subscale of the self-report habit index. International Journal of Behavioral Nutiriton and Physical Activity, 9, 102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haggard, P. (2005). Conscious intention and motor cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 290–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H., & Beckmann, J. (1990). Intentional action and action slips. Psychological Review, 97, 36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, A. M. C., & Garavan, H. (2005). Human functional neuroimaging of brain changes associated with practice. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1089–1102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marien, H., Aarts, H., & Custers, R. (2013). Adaptive control of human action: The role of outcome representations and reward signals. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marien, H., Aarts, H., & Custers, R. (2015). The interactive role of action-outcome learning and positive affective information in motivating human goal-directed behavior. Motivation Science, 1, 165–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. R., & Varela, F. G. (1987). The tree of knowledge. Boston: Shambhala.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the organization of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention and action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 4, pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Doherty, J. P. (2004). Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 769–776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 54–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., Johnston, J. C., & Ruthruff, E. (2001). Attention and performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 629–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plato. (380BC/1986). Protagoras. In B. A. F. Hubbard, & E. S. Karnofsky (Trans.), The dialogues of Plato. New York: Bantam Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. (1979). Actions not as planned: The price of automatization. In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (Eds.), Aspects of consciousness (pp. 67–89). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1976). Stimulus generalization: Some predictions from a model of Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 88–96.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1990). The role of information about the response-outcome relation in instrumental discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 262–270.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. Oxford: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R., Knowlton, B., & Musen, G. (1993). The structure and organization of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 453–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1933). A proof of the law of effect. Science, 77, 173–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trapold, M. A. (1970). Are expectancies based upon different positive reinforcing events discriminably different? Learning and Motivation, 1, 129–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A., & Moonen, A. (1998). Habit versus planned behavior: A field experiment. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 111–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A., & van Knippenberg, C. (1994). Attitude versus general habit: Antecedents of travel mode choice. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 285–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., & Orbell, S. (2003). Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(6), 1313–1330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1925). Behaviorism. New York: The People’s Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, P., Wiers, R. W., Hommel, B., & de Wit, S. (2014). Working for food you don’t desire. Cues interfere with goal-directed food-seeking. Appetite, 79, 139–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Control and automaticity in social life. In D. T. Gilbert & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, 4th ed., pp. 446–496). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics. Scientific American, 179, 14–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zedelius, C. M., Veling, H., & Aarts, H. (2011). Boosting or choking—How conscious and unconscious reward processing modulate the active maintenance of goal-relevant information. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 355–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zedelius, C. M., Veling, H., Custers, R., Bijleveld, E., Chiew, K. S., & Aarts, H. (2014). A new perspective on human reward research: How consciously and unconsciously perceived reward information influences performance. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 493–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hans Marien .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Marien, H., Custers, R., Aarts, H. (2018). Understanding the Formation of Human Habits: An Analysis of Mechanisms of Habitual Behaviour. In: Verplanken, B. (eds) The Psychology of Habit. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-97529-0_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics