Advertisement

Defining Habit in Psychology

  • Asaf Mazar
  • Wendy Wood
Chapter

Abstract

We review definitions of habit and evaluate how well current measures capture these definitions. Habits are context–behaviour associations in memory that develop as people repeatedly experience rewards for a given action in a given context. Habitual behaviour is cued directly by context and does not require supporting goals and conscious intentions. As we explain, goals are important to initiate behaviour repetition and to inhibit unwanted habit performance, and in addition can be inferred from habits. To date, research has relied on self-report methods to measure habit. These measures, by themselves, may fall short of adequately testing key features of habit, such as context dependence, or dissociating habitual and non-habitual influences on behaviour. By augmenting self-reports with implicit, behavioural, and ecological momentary assessment methods, researchers can gain a more complete understanding of habits and their role in shaping behaviour.

Keywords

Habit Automatic processes Implicit Habit definition Habit measurement 

References

  1. Aarts, H. (2007). Health and goal-directed behavior: The nonconscious regulation and motivation of goals and their pursuit. Health Psychology Review, 1(1), 53–82.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17437190701485852CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adams, C. D. (1982). Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding to reinforcer devaluation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 34(2), 77–98.  https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748208400878CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Adriaanse, M. A., Prinsen, S., de Witt Huberts, J. C., de Ridder, D. T. D., & Evers, C. (2016). ‘I ate too much so I must have been sad’: Emotions as a confabulated reason for overeating. Appetite, 103, 318–323.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.04.028CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Ajzen, I. (2002). Residual effects of past on later behavior: Habituation and reasoned action perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6(2), 107–122.  https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0602_02CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Aldrich, J. H., Montgomery, J. M., & Wood, W. (2011). Turnout as a habit. Political Behavior, 33(4), 535–563.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-010-9148-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Anderson, B. A. (2016). The attention habit: How reward learning shapes attentional selection. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1369, 24–39.  https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12957CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bargh, J. A. (2006). What have we been priming all these years? On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of nonconscious social behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36(2), 147–168.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.336CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Bargh, J. A. (2013). Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes. Hove: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 6, 1–62.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60024-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Botvinick, M., & Plaut, D. C. (2004). Doing without schema hierarchies: A recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. Psychological Review, 111, 395–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Danner, U. N., Aarts, H., & de Vries, N. K. (2008). Habit vs. intention in the prediction of future behaviour: The role of frequency, context stability and mental accessibility of past behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47(2), 245–265.  https://doi.org/10.1348/014466607X230876CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., & Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 347–368.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014211CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. de Wit, S., & Dickinson, A. (2009). Associative theories of goal-directed behaviour: a case for animal–human translational models. Psychological Research PRPF, 73(4), 463–476.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0230-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dickinson, A. (1985). Actions and habits: The development of behavioural autonomy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, 308(1135), 67–78.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1985.0010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  17. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2011). Predicting and changing behavior: The reasoned action approach. Taylor & Francis. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Foerde, K. (2018). What are habits and do they depend on the striatum? A view from the study of neuropsychological populations. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 20, 17–24.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.08.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Galla, B. M., & Duckworth, A. L. (2015). More than resisting temptation: Beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(3), 508–525.  https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000026CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Gardner, B. (2009). Modelling motivation and habit in stable travel mode contexts. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 12(1), 68–76.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2008.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gardner, B. (2015). A review and analysis of the use of ‘habit’ in understanding, predicting and influencing health-related behaviour. Health Psychology Review, 9(3), 277–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gardner, B., Abraham, C., Lally, P., & de Bruijn, G.-J. (2012). Towards parsimony in habit measurement: Testing the convergent and predictive validity of an automaticity subscale of the self-report habit index. The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 9, 102.  https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-9-102CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Gardner, B., & Tang, V. (2014). Reflecting on non-reflective action: An exploratory think-aloud study of self-report habit measures. British Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 258–273.  https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12060CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Gershman, S. J., Gerstenberg, T., Baker, C. L., & Cushman, F. A. (2016). Plans, habits, and theory of mind. PLoS One, 11(9), e0162246.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0162246CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Gillan, C. M., Otto, A. R., Phelps, E. A., & Daw, N. D. (2015). Model-based learning protects against forming habits. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(3), 523–536.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0347-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hagger, M. S., Rebar, A. L., Mullan, B., Lipp, O. V., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2015). The subjective experience of habit captured by self-report indexes may lead to inaccuracies in the measurement of habitual action. Health Psychology Review, 9(3), 296–302.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2014.959728CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory. Oxford: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
  28. Itzchakov, G., Uziel, L., & Wood, W. (2018). When attitudes and habits don’t correspond: Self-control depletion increases persuasion but not behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 75, 1–10.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.10.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
  30. James, W. (1916/1983). Talks to teachers on psychology and to students on some of life’s ideals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Ji, M. F., & Wood, W. (2007). Purchase and consumption habits: Not necessarily what you intend. Journal of Consumer Psychology (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 17(4), 261–276.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10577400701542403CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Knowlton, B. J., & Patterson, T. K. (2016). Habit formation and the striatum. In Behavioral neuroscience of learning and memory (pp. 275–295). Cham: Springer.  https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2016_451CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W. Y., & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal systems. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 331–378). San Diego: Academic Press. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0065260102800089Google Scholar
  34. Labrecque, J. S., Lee, K., & Wood, W. (in preparation). Overthinking habit. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
  35. Labrecque, J. S., & Wood, W. (2015). What measures of habit strength to use? Comment on Gardner (2015). Health Psychology Review, 9(3), 303–310.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2014.992030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Loersch, C., & Payne, B. K. (2011). The situated inference model: An integrative account of the effects of primes on perception, behavior, and motivation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(3), 234–252.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611406921CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492–527.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Mantzari, E., Vogt, F., Shemilt, I., Wei, Y., Higgins, J. P. T., & Marteau, T. M. (2015). Personal financial incentives for changing habitual health-related behaviors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Preventive Medicine, 75, 75–85.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.03.001CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Marhe, R., Waters, A. J., van de Wetering, B. J. M., & Franken, I. H. A. (2013). Implicit and explicit drug-related cognitions during detoxification treatment are associated with drug relapse: An ecological momentary assessment study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81(1), 1–12.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030754CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Mazar, A. S., Lieberman, A., Wood, W., & Itzchakov, G. (in preparation). Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
  41. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(2), 297–326.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Labrecque, J. S., & Lally, P. (2012). How do habits guide behavior? Perceived and actual triggers of habits in daily life. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(2), 492–498.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.10.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Wu, M., & Kurlander, D. (2011). The pull of the past when do habits persist despite conflict with motives? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(11), 1428–1437.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211419863CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Orbell, S., & Verplanken, B. (2010). The automatic component of habit in health behavior: Habit as cue-contingent automaticity. Health Psychology, 29(4), 374–383.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019596CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124(1), 54–74.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sabin, J. A., Marini, M., & Nosek, B. A. (2012). Implicit and explicit anti-fat Bias among a large sample of medical doctors by BMI, race/ethnicity and gender. PLoS One, 7(11), e48448.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048448CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  48. Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (2014). Dual-process theories of the social mind. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  49. Shiffman, S., Hufford, M., Hickcox, M., Paty, J. A., Gnys, M., & Kassel, J. D. (1997). Remember that? A comparison of real-time versus retrospective recall of smoking lapses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(2), 292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
  52. Sniehotta, F. F., & Presseau, J. (2012). The habitual use of the self-report habit index. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 43(1), 139–140. author reply 141-2.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-011-9305-xCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Squire, L. R., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science, 253(5026), 1380–1386.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1896849CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Stone, A. A., Shiffman, S. S., & DeVries, M. W. (1999). Ecological momentary assessment. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 26–39). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
  55. Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–324.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.xCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Thomas, G. O., Poortinga, W., & Sautkina, E. (2016). Habit discontinuity, self-activation, and the diminishing influence of context change: Evidence from the UK understanding society survey. PLoS One, 11(4), e0153490.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0153490CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. The Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements, 2(4), i.Google Scholar
  58. Thrailkill, E. A., & Bouton, M. E. (2015). Contextual control of instrumental actions and habits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 41(1), 69–80.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000045CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55(4), 189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Triandis, H. C. (1977). Interpersonal behavior. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks-Cole.Google Scholar
  61. Triandis, H. C. (1980). Values, attitudes, and interpersonal behavior. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 27, 195–259.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Tricomi, E., Balleine, B. W., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2009). A specific role for posterior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29(11), 2225–2232.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06796.xCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., & van Knippenberg, A. (1997). Habit, information acquisition, and the process of making travel mode choices. European Journal of Social Psychology, 27(5), 539–560.  https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199709/10)27:5<539::AID-EJSP831>3.0.CO;2-ACrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A., & Moonen, A. (1998). Habit versus planned behavior: A field experiment. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37(1), 111–128.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1998.tb01160.xCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Verplanken, B., & Orbell, S. (2003). Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(6), 1313–1330.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Verplanken, B., & Roy, D. (2016). Empowering interventions to promote sustainable lifestyles: Testing the habit discontinuity hypothesis in a field experiment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 45, 127–134.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.11.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Verplanken, B., Walker, I., Davis, A., & Jurasek, M. (2008). Context change and travel mode choice: Combining the habit discontinuity and self-activation hypotheses. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(2), 121–127.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.10.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Waters, A. J., Marhe, R., & Franken, I. H. A. (2012). Attentional bias to drug cues is elevated before and during temptations to use heroin and cocaine. Psychopharmacology, 219(3), 909–921.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2424-zCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Watson, P., & de Wit, S. (2018). Current limits of experimental research into habits and future directions. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 20, 33–39.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.09.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2009). The habitual consumer. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19, 579–592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2016). Healthy through habit: Interventions for initiating & maintaining health behavior change. Behavioral Science & Policy, 2(1), 71–83.  https://doi.org/10.1353/bsp.2016.0008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Wood, W., Quinn, J. M., & Kashy, D. A. (2002). Habits in everyday life: Thought, emotion, and action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1281–1297.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1281CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  73. Wood, W., & Rünger, D. (2016). Psychology of habit. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 289–314.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033417CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Wood, W., Tam, L., & Witt, M. G. (2005). Changing circumstances, disrupting habits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(6), 918–933.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.918CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Yin, H. H., & Knowlton, B. J. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(6), 464–476.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1919CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Asaf Mazar
    • 1
  • Wendy Wood
    • 2
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Southern CaliforniaLos AngelesUSA
  2. 2.Department of Psychology and Marshall School of BusinessUniversity of Southern CaliforniaLos AngelesUSA
  3. 3.Marketing, INSEADFranceSingapore

Personalised recommendations