Abstract
It is well established in the literature on deception that most individuals are not very good at detecting lies. Instead, humans tend to be biased toward truthfulness. In this chapter, we review socio-cognitive processes underlying our judgments of truth and deception. We examine how the development of our species, the development of our own minds, and the development of our sociocultural structures all work toward creating a balance between abilities to lie, and the abilities to detect lies. We suggest that our social networks serve as a deception surveillance system that will usually work to produce the concrete proof that a lie was told, thus removing the evolutionary press for us all to become excellent lie catchers from behavioral responses alone. Finally, as our social world expands, social media has kept up to move the social surveillance system from the level of the village, to the level of the world, as illustrated by some new phenomena like the #MeToo movement.
Keywords
- Deception detection
- Evolution
- Deception
- Lying
- Social cognition
- Theory of Mind
- Cooperation
- Free rider
- Truth bias
- #MeToo
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
Buying options
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Learn about institutional subscriptionsReferences
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,108, 441–485. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.108.4.441.
Astington, J. W. (1993). The child’s discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Aune, R. K., & Waters, L. L. (1994). Cultural differences in deception: Motivations to deceive in Samoans and North Americans. International Journal of Intercultural Relations,18, 159–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/0147-1767(94)90026-4.
Baker, A., ten Brinke, L., & Porter, S. (2013). Will get fooled again: Emotionally intelligent people are easily duped by high-stakes deceivers. Legal and Criminological Psychology,18, 300–313. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8333.2012.02054.x.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Evolution of a theory of mind? In M. C. Corballis & S. E. G. Lea (Eds.), The descent of mind: Psychological perspectives on hominid evolution (pp. 261–277). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Bear, A., & Rand, D. G. (2016). Intuition, deliberation, and the evolution of cooperation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,113, 936–941. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1517780113.
Bee, M. A., Perrill, S. A., & Owen, P. C. (2000). Male green frogs lower the pitch of acoustic signals in defense of territories: A possible dishonest signal of size? Behavioral Ecology,11, 169–177. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/11.2.169.
Boehm, C. (1993). Egalitarian behavior and reverse dominance hierarchy. Current Anthropology,34(3), 227–254.
Bond, G. D. (2008). Deception detection expertise. Law and Human Behavior,32, 339–351. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-007-9110-z.
Bond, Jr., C. F., & DePaulo, B. M. (2006). Accuracy of deception judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 214–234. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2.
Bond, C. F., Omar, A., Mahmoud, A., & Bonser, R. N. (1990). Lie detection across cultures. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,14, 189–204. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00996226.
Bond, C. F., & Robinson, M. (1988). The evolution of deception. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,12, 295–307. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00987597.
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary foundation of religious belief. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Buller, D. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1996). Interpersonal deception theory. Communication Theory,6, 203–242. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.1996.tb00127.x.
Burkart, J. M., Allon, O., Amici, F., Fichtel, C., Finkenwirth, C., Heschl, A., …, van Schaik, C. P. (2014). The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation. Nature Communications, 5, 4747. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms5747.
Bussey, K. (1999). Children’s categorization and evaluation of different types of lies and truths. Child Development,70, 1338–1347. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00098.
Bussey, K., & Grimbeek, E. J. (2000). Children’s conceptions of lying and truth-telling: Implications for child witnesses. Legal and Criminological Psychology,5, 187–199. https://doi.org/10.1348/135532500168083.
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1988). Toward the next generation in data quality: A new survey of primate tactical deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,11, 267–273. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00049955.
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1991). Computation and mindreading in primate tactical deception. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading (pp. 127–141). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1992). Cognitive evolution in primates: Evidence from tactical deception. Man,27, 609–627. https://doi.org/10.2307/2803931.
Chance, Z., & Norton, M. I. (2015). The what and why of self-deception. Current Opinion in Psychology,6, 104–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.07.008.
Chandler, M., Fritz, A. S., & Hala, S. (1989). Small-scale deceit: Deception as a marker of two-, three-, and four-year-olds’ early theories of mind. Child Development,60, 1263–1277. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130919.
Cheung, H., Siu, T. S. C., & Chen, L. (2015). The roles of liar intention, lie content, and theory of mind in children’s evaluation of lies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,132, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.12.002.
Cook, L. M., Grant, B. S., Saccheri, I. J., & Mallet, J. (2012). Selective bird predation on the peppered moth: The last experiment of Michael Majerus. Biology Letters,8, 609–612. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2011.1136.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1989). Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, part II: Case study: A computational theory of social exchange. Ethology and Sociobiology,10, 51–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(89)90013-7.
Courtland, S. D. (2015). Detecting animal deception. The Journal of Mind and Behavior,36(3/4), 121–138.
Cummins, R. A., & Nistico, H. (2002). Maintaining life satisfaction: The role of positive cognitive bias. Journal of Happiness Studies,3, 37–69. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015678915305.
Danziger, E. (2010). On trying and lying: Cultural configurations of Grice’s Maxim of Quality. Intercultural Pragmatics,7, 199–219. https://doi.org/10.1515/iprg.2010.010.
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Dawkins, R., & Krebs, J. R. (1979). Arms races between and within species. Proceedings of the Royal Society B,205, 489–511. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1979.0081.
DePaulo, B. M., Kashy, D. A., Kirkendol, S. E., Wyer, M. M., & Epstein, J. A. (1996). Lying in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,70, 979–995. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.979.
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin,129(1), 74–118.
De Waal, F. B. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology,59, 279–300. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625.
Dor, D. (2017). The role of the lie in the evolution of human language. Language Sciences,63, 44–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2017.01.001.
Dunbar, R. I. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,16, 681–694. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00032325.
Dunbar, R. I. (2004). Gossip in evolutionary perspective. Review of General Psychology,8, 100–110. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.8.2.100.
Dunbar, N. E., Gangi, K., Coveleski, S., Adams, A., Bernhold, Q., & Giles, H. (2016). When is it acceptable to lie?: Interpersonal and intergroup perspectives on deception. Communication Studies,67, 129–146. https://doi.org/10.1080/10510974.2016.1146911.
Efron, D. (1941). Gesture and environment. New York, NY: Kings Crown Press.
Ekman, P. (1985/2001). Telling lies: Clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics, and marriage. New York, NY: Norton.
Ekman, P. (1996). Why don’t we catch liars? Social Research,63(3), 801–817.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica,1, 49–98. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1969.1.1.49.
Ekman, P., & O’Sullivan, M. (1991). Who can catch a liar? American Psychologist,46, 913–920. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.46.9.913.
Ekman, P., O’Sullivan, M., & Frank, M. G. (1999). A few can catch a liar. Psychological Science,10, 263–266. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00147.
Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Five-year olds, but not chimpanzees, attempt to manage their reputations. PLoS One, 7. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048433.
Enquist, M., & Leimar, O. (1993). The evolution of cooperation in mobile organisms. Animal Behaviour,45, 747–757. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1993.1089.
Etcoff, N. L., Ekman, P., Magee, J. J., & Frank, M. G. (2000). Lie detection and language comprehension. Nature,405, 139. https://doi.org/10.1038/35012129.
Feinberg, M., Willer, R., Stellar, J., & Keltner, D. (2012). The virtues of gossip: Reputational information sharing as prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,102, 1015–1030. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026650.
Fitch, W. T. (2010). The evolution of language. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Frank, M. G., & Feeley, T. H. (2003). To catch a liar: Challenges for research in lie detection training. Journal of Applied Communication Research,31, 58–75. https://doi.org/10.1080/00909880305377.
Frank, M. G., & Svetieva, E. (2013). The role of nonverbal behavior in detecting and telling lies. In M. L. Knapp & J. A. Hall (Eds.), Nonverbal communication (pp. 471–511). New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter.
Franzen, A., & Pointner, S. (2012). Anonymity in the dictator game revisited. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization,81, 74–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.09.005.
Freeman, D. M. A. (2009). Sociocultural perspectives on dishonesty and lying. In S. Akhtar & H. Parens (Eds.), Lying, cheating, and carrying on: Developmental, clinical, and sociocultural aspects of dishonesty and deceit (pp. 111–127). New York, NY: Jason Aronson.
Friesen, W. V. (1972). Cultural differences in facial expressions in a social situation: An experimental test of the concept of display rules (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California, San Francisco.
Fu, G., Evans, A. D., Wang, L., & Lee, K. (2008). Lying in the name of the collective good: A developmental study. Developmental Science,11, 495–503. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00695.x.
Gavrilets, S., & Richerson, P. J. (2017). Collective action and the evolution of social norm internalization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,114, 6068–6073. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1703857114.
Gilsenan, M. (1976). Lying, honor, and contradiction. In B. Kapferer (Ed.),Transaction and meaning: Directions in the anthropology of exchange and symbolic behavior (ASA Essays in Social Anthropology Series, Vol. 1) (pp. 191–219). Philadelphia, PA: Institute for the Study of Human Issues.
Gintis, H. (2000). Strong reciprocity and human sociality. Journal of Theoretical Biology,206, 169–179. https://doi.org/10.1006/jtbi.2000.2111.
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics. Volume 3: Speech acts (pp. 225–242). New York, NY: Seminar Press.
Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review,108, 814–834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814.
Haley, K. J., & Fessler, D. M. (2005). Nobody’s watching?: Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior,26, 245–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.01.002.
Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. New York, NY: Doubleday.
Hall, K., & Brosnan, S. F. (2017). Cooperation and deception in primates. Infant Behavior and Development,48, 38–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.11.007.
Hauser, M. D. (1992). Articulatory and social factors influence the acoustic structure of rhesus monkey vocalizations: A learned mode of production? The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,91, 2175–2179. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.403676.
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., Shachat, K., & Smith, V. (1994). Preferences, property rights, and anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior,7, 346–380. https://doi.org/10.1006/game.1994.1056.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P. Bateson & R. Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303–317). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Jensen, K. (2010). Punishment and spite, the dark side of cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,365, 2635–2650. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0146.
Jensen, K., Vaish, A., & Schmidt, M. F. (2014). The emergence of human prosociality: Aligning with others through feelings, concerns, and norms. Frontiers in Psychology,5, 822. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00822.
Jonas, E., Schulz-Hardt, S., Frey, D., & Thelen, N. (2001). Confirmation bias in sequential information search after preliminary decisions: An expansion of dissonance theoretical research on selective exposure to information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,80, 557–571. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.557.
Klein, N., & Epley, N. (2015). Group discussion improves lie detection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,112, 7460–7465. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1504048112.
Knapp, M. L. (2008). Lying and deception in human interaction. Boston, MA: Pearson Education.
Knapp, M. L., & Comadena, M. E. (1979). Telling it like it isn’t: A review of theory and research on deceptive communications. Human Communication Research,5, 270–285. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1979.tb00640.x.
Knight, C. (1991). Blood relations: Menstruation and the origins of culture. London, UK and New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Knight, C. (1998). Ritual/speech coevolution: A solution to the problem of deception. In J. R. Hurfurd, M. Studdert-Kennedy, & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language: Social and cognitive bases (pp. 68–91). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Knight, C. (2008). Language co-evolved with the rule of law. Mind & Society,7, 109–128. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-007-0039-1.
Krebs, J. R., & Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behaviouralecology: An evolutionary approach (pp. 380–402). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science,9, 178–181. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00088.
Leduc, K., Williams, S., Gomez-Garibello, C., & Talwar, V. (2017). The contributions of mental state understanding and executive functioning to preschool-aged children’s lie-telling. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,35, 288–302. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12163.
Lee, K. (2000). Lying as doing deceptive things with words: A speech act theoretical perspective. In J. W. Astington (Ed.), Mind in the making (pp. 177–196). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Lee, K. (2013). Little liars: Development of verbal deception in children. Child Development Perspectives,7, 91–96. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12023.
Lee, K., & Ross, H. J. (1997). The concept of lying in adolescents and young adults: Testing Sweetser’s folkloristic model. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,43(2), 255–270.
Levine, T. R. (2014). Truth-default theory (TDT): A theory of human deception and deception detection. Journal of Language and Social Psychology,33, 378–392. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X14535916.
Lyons, M., Healy, N., & Bruno, D. (2013). It takes one to know one: Relationship between lie detection and psychopathy. Personality and Individual Differences,55, 676–679. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.05.018.
Ma, F., Evans, A. D., Liu, Y., Luo, X., & Xu, F. (2015). To lie or not to lie? The influence of parenting and theory-of-mind understanding on three-year-old children’s honesty. Journal of Moral Education,44, 198–212. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2015.1023182.
Mascaro, O., Morin, O., & Sperber, D. (2017). Optimistic expectations about communication explain children’s difficulties in hiding, lying, and mistrusting liars. Journal of Child Language,44, 1041–1064. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000350.
McCornack, S. A., & Parks, M. R. (1986). Deception detection and relationship development: The other side of trust. Annals of the International Communication Association,9, 377–389. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.1986.11678616.
McCrone, J. (1991). The ape that spoke: Language and the evolution of the human mind. New York, NY: William Morrow & Co.
McNally, L., & Jackson, A. L. (2013). Cooperation creates selection for tactical deception. Proceedings of the Royal Society B,280, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.0699.
Mealy, M., Stephan, W., & Urrutia, I. C. (2007). The acceptability of lies: A comparison of Ecuadorians and Euro-Americans. International Journal of Intercultural Relations,31, 689–702. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2007.06.002.
Mitchell, R. W. (1986). A framework for discussing deception. In R. W. Mitchell & N. S. Thompson (Eds.), Deception: Perspectives on human and nonhuman deceit (pp. 3–40). Albany: State University of New York Press.
Mitchell, R. W. (1987). A comparative-developmental approach to understanding imitation. In P. P. G. Bateson & P. H. Klopfer (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology (pp. 183–215). Boston, MA: Springer.
Mitchell, R. W. (1993). Animals as liars: The human face of nonhuman duplicity. In M. Lewis & C. Saarni (Eds.), Lying and deception in everyday life (pp. 59–89). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Newton, P., Reddy, V., & Bull, R. (2000). Children’s everyday deception and performance on false-belief tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,18, 297–317. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151000165706.
Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology,2, 175–220. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175.
Ning, S. R., & Crossman, A. M. (2007). We believe in being honest: Examining subcultural differences in the acceptability of deception. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,37, 2130–2155. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00254.x.
Novotny, E., Carr, Z., Frank, M. G., Dietrich, S. B., Shaddock, T., Cardwell, M., & Decker, A. (2018). How people really suspect and discover lies. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 42, 41–52. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0263-2.
O’Sullivan, M. (2003). The fundamental attribution error in detecting deceit: The boy-who-cried-wolf effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,29, 1316–1327. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203254610.
O’Sullivan, M., & Ekman, P. (2004). The wizards of deception detection. In P. A. Granhag & L. Strömwell (Eds.), The detection of deception in forensic contexts (pp. 269–286). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
O’Sullivan, M., Frank, M. G., Hurley, C. M., & Tiwana, J. (2009). Police lie detection accuracy: The effect of lie scenario. Law and Human Behavior,33, 530–538. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9166-4.
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5-to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,39, 437–471. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7.
Piaget, J. (1932/1965). The moral judgement of the child. London, UK: Free Press.
Premack, D. (2007). Human and animal cognition: Continuity and discontinuity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,104, 13861–13867. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0706147104.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences,1, 515–526. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512.
Rauwolf, P. (2016). Understanding the ubiquity of self-deception: The evolutionary utility of incorrect information (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Bath, UK.
Rauwolf, P., Mitchell, D., & Bryson, J. J. (2015). Value homophily benefits cooperation but motivates employing incorrect social information. Journal of Theoretical Biology,367, 246–261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2014.11.023.
Riggio, R. E., Tucker, J., & Throckmorton, B. (1987). Social skills and deception ability. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,13, 568–577. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167287134013.
Rodino-Colocino, M. (2018). Me too, #MeToo: Countering cruelty with empathy. Communication and Critical/Cultural Studies,15, 96–100. https://doi.org/10.1080/14791420.2018.1435083.
Saarni, C., & Weber, H. (1999). Emotional displays and dissemblance in childhood: Implications for self presentation. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & E. Coates (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 71–105). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Santos, L. R., & Rosati, A. G. (2015). The evolutionary roots of human decision making. Annual Review of Psychology,66, 321–347. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015310.
Schulz-Hardt, S., Frey, D., Lüthgens, C., & Moscovici, S. (2000). Biased information search in group decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,78, 655–669. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.655.
Serota, K. B., & Levine, T. R. (2015). A few prolific liars: Variation in the prevalence of lying. Journal of Language and Social Psychology,34, 138–157. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X14528804.
Shaw, J., Porter, S., & Ten Brinke, L. (2013). Catching liars: Training mental health and legal professionals to detect high-stakes lies. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology,24, 145–159. https://doi.org/10.1080/14789949.2012.752025.
Sip, K. E., Roepstorff, A., McGregor, W., & Frith, C. D. (2008). Detecting deception: the scope and limits. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,12, 48–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.11.008.
Sodian, B., & Frith, U. (1992). Deception and sabotage in autistic, retarded and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,33, 591–605. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00893.x.
Sommerfeld, R. D., Krambeck, H. J., & Milinski, M. (2008). Multiple gossip statements and their effect on reputation and trustworthiness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences,275, 2529–2536. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0762.
Sowden, S., Wright, G. R., Banissy, M. J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2015). Transcranial current stimulation of the temporoparietal junction improves lie detection. Current Biology,25, 2447–2451. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.08.014.
Spence, S. A., Hunter, M. D., Farrow, T. F., Green, R. D., Leung, D. H., Hughes, C. J., & Ganesan, V. (2004). A cognitive neurobiological account of deception: Evidence from functional neuroimaging. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 359, 1755–1762. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2004.1555.
Stevens, J. R. (2008). The evolutionary biology of decision making. In C. Engel & W. Singer (Eds.), Better than conscious? Decision making, the human mind, and implications for institutions (pp. 285–304). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Street, C. N. (2015). ALIED: Humans as adaptive lie detectors. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition,4, 335–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2015.06.002.
Suddendorf, T., & Whiten, A. (2001). Mental evolution and development: Evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes, and other animals. Psychological Bulletin,127, 629–650. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.127.5.629.
Sweetser, E. (1987). The definition of lie. In D. Holland & N. Quinn (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 43–66). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Sztompka, P. (1999). Trust: A sociological theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Talwar, V., & Lee, K. (2002). Development of lying to conceal a transgression: Children’s control of expressive behaviour during verbal deception. International Journal of Behavioral Development,26, 436–444. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250143000373.
Talwar, V., & Lee, K. (2008). Social and cognitive correlates of children’s lying behavior. Child Development,79, 866–881. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01164.x.
Talwar, V., Williams, S. M., Renaud, S. J., Arruda, C., & Saykaly, C. (2016). Children’s evaluations of tattles, confessions, prosocial and antisocial lies. International Review of Pragmatics,8, 334–352. https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-00802007.
Tomasello, M. (1999). Having intentions, understanding intentions, and understanding communicative intentions. In P. D. Zelazo, J. W. Astington, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control (pp. 63–75). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). In search of the uniquely human. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,28, 721–727. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05540123.
Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology,53, 673–692. https://doi.org/10.1086/668207.
Towner, S. (2010). Concept of mind in non-human primates. Bioscience Horizons,3, 96–104. https://doi.org/10.1093/biohorizons/hzq011.
Trivers, R. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology,46, 35–57. https://doi.org/10.1086/406755.
Trivers, R. (1985). Social evolution. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings Publishers.
Trivers, R. (1991). Deceit and self-deception: The relationship between communication and consciousness. In M. Robinson & L. Tiger (Eds.), Man and beast revisited (pp. 175–191). Washington, DC: Smithsonian.
Tucker, A. W. (1950). A two-person dilemma (Unpublished manuscript). Stanford University, Stanford, CA.
Van Valen, L. (1973). A new evolutionary law. Evolutionary Theory,1, 1–30.
Von Hippel, W., & Trivers, R. (2011). Reflections on self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,34, 41–56. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10003018.
Vrij, A. (2007). Deception: A social lubricant and a selfish act. In K. Fiedler (Ed.), Frontiers of social psychology: Social communication (pp. 309–342). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Vrij, A., Dragt, A., & Koppelaar, L. (1992). Interviews with ethnic interviewees: Non-verbal communication errors in impression formation. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology,2, 199–208. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.2450020304.
Wellman, H. M. (1992). The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Wellman, H. M., & Bartsch, K. (1988). Young children’s reasoning about beliefs. Cognition,30, 239–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(88)90021-2.
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development,75, 523–541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x.
Whiten, A., & Byrne, R. W. (1988). Tactical deception in primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,11, 233–244. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00049682.
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,364, 2417–2428. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0069.
Willer, R., Feinberg, M., Irwin, K., Schultz, M., & Simpson, B. (2010). The trouble with invisible men: How reputational concerns motivate generosity. In S. Hitlin & S. Vaisey (Eds.), Handbook of the sociology of morality (pp. 315–330). New York, NY: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-6896-8_17.
Williams, S., Moore, K., Crossman, A. M., & Talwar, V. (2016). The role of executive functions and theory of mind in children’s prosocial lie-telling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,141, 256–266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.001.
Wilson, A. E., Smith, M. D., & Ross, H. S. (2003). The nature and effects of young children’s lies. Social Development,12, 21–45. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00220.
Wilson, D. S., Wilczynski, C., Wells, A., & Weiser, L. (2000). Gossip and other aspects of language as group-level adaptations. In L. Huber & C. Heyes (Eds.), The evolution of cognition (pp. 347–365). Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition,13, 103–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5.
Wright, G. R., Berry, C. J., & Bird, G. (2012). “You can’t kid a kidder”: Association between production and detection of deception in an interactive deception task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,6, 87. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00087.
Xu, F., Bao, X., Fu, G., Talwar, V., & Lee, K. (2010). Lying and truth-telling in children: From concept to action. Child Development,81, 581–596. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01417.x.
Xu, F., Luo, Y. C., Fu, G., & Lee, K. (2009). Children’s and adults’ conceptualization and evaluation of lying and truth-telling. Infant and Child Development,18, 307–322. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.631.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2019 The Author(s)
About this chapter
Cite this chapter
Solbu, A., Frank, M.G. (2019). Lie Catchers: Evolution and Development of Deception in Modern Times. In: Docan-Morgan, T. (eds) The Palgrave Handbook of Deceptive Communication. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-96334-1_3
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-96334-1_3
Published:
Publisher Name: Palgrave Macmillan, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-319-96333-4
Online ISBN: 978-3-319-96334-1
eBook Packages: Behavioral Science and PsychologyBehavioral Science and Psychology (R0)