Advertisement

The Infamous Relationship Between Violent Video Game Use and Aggression: Uncharted Moderators and Small Effects Make It a Far Cry from Certain

  • Aaron DrummondEmail author
  • James D. Sauer
  • Shaun S. Garea
Chapter

Abstract

There is ongoing scientific and public interest in whether playing violent video games (VVGs) increases aggression. Here, we review the extant evidence for the effects of VVGs on aggression across three different types of research – (1) experimental, lab-based studies, (2) cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, and (3) epidemiological studies of societal violence (e.g. violent crime rates). Although the idea that playing VVGs causes aggression is intuitively appealing, empirical evidence suggests that strong claims about a positive relationship are unjustified. In the laboratory, playing VVGs leads to only small increases in aggression, and, when issues such as publication bias are accounted for, these estimates decline further in size (Hilgard et al. Psychological Bulletin 143:757, 2017). In terms of societal violence, the evidence consistently shows no relationship between VVG use and violence, sometimes even showing decreases in violence associated with increased VVG use (e.g. Markey, Markey & French, 2015). Evidence further suggests that the relationship between VVGs and aggression is not well understood, with factors such as narrative context, game difficulty, and competitiveness potentially influencing or accounting for any relationship between gameplay and aggression. Some studies suggest that, under some circumstances, VVGs may even reduce hostility (e.g. when used for mood management; Ferguson & Rueda (European Psychologist 15:99, 2010). At present, we feel the evidence weighs against the notion that VVGs cause aggression, though more sophisticated theoretical and methodological approaches are necessary to more fully understand the boundary conditions of the relationship.

Keywords

Violent Video Games Aggression General Aggression Model Societal Violence Media Violence Media Psychology 

References

  1. ABS. (2017). 4510.0 Recorded crime – victims, Australia, 2016. Retrieved 20 Dec 2017. http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/mf/4510.0/
  2. Adachi, P. J., & Willoughby, T. (2011a). The effect of video game competition and violence on aggressive behavior: Which characteristic has the greatest influence? Psychology of Violence, 1(4), 259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Adachi, P. J., & Willoughby, T. (2011b). The effect of violent video games on aggression: Is it more than just the violence? Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16(1), 55–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. AIC. (2017). Facts & figures online data tool. Victims of violent crime (n per year), 2016. Retrieved 20 Dec 2017. http://www.aic.gov.au/dataTools/facts/vicViolentCol.html
  5. Allen, J. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2017). Aggression and violence: Definitions and distinctions. In P. Sturmey (Ed) The Wiley handbook of violence and aggression. West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
  6. Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: A meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychological Science, 12(5), 353–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Anderson, C. A., Bushman, B. J., Bartholow, B. D., Cantor, J., Christakis, D., Coyne, S. M., … Green, C. S. (2017). Screen violence and youth behavior. Pediatrics, 140(Supplement 2), S142–S147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Anderson, C. A., & Carnagey, N. L. (2004). Violent evil and the general aggression model. In The social psychology of good and evil (pp. 168–192). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  10. Anderson, C. A., & Carnagey, N. L. (2009). Causal effects of violent sports video games on aggression: Is it competitiveness or violent content? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 731–739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Anderson, C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000). Video games and aggressive thoughts, feelings, and behavior in the laboratory and in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Anderson, C. A., Sakamoto, A., Gentile, D. A., Ihori, N., Shibuya, A., Yukawa, S., … Kobayashi, K. (2008). Longitudinal effects of violent video games on aggression in Japan and the United States. Pediatrics, 122(5), e1067–e1072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., … Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in eastern and western countries: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 151–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory of mass communication. Media Psychology, 3(3), 265–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Barlett, C. P., Anderson, C. A., & Swing, E. L. (2009). Video game effects—Confirmed, suspected, and speculative: A review of the evidence. Simulation & Gaming, 40(3), 377–403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Breuer, J., Scharkow, M., & Quandt, T. (2015). Sore losers? A reexamination of the frustration–aggression hypothesis for colocated video game play. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 4(2), 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Bushman, B. J. (1995). Moderating role of trait aggressiveness in the effects of violent media on aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 950–950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2002). Violent video games and hostile expectations: A test of the general aggression model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(12), 1679–1686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Carnagey, N. L. (2006). Is it competitiveness or violent content? The effects of violent sports video games on aggression (Doctoral Thesis). Iowa State University, Iowa, USA.Google Scholar
  20. Carnagey, N. L., & Anderson, C. A. (2005). The effects of reward and punishment in violent video games on aggressive affect, cognition, and behavior. Psychological Science, 16(11), 882–889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Çetin, Y., Wai, J., Altay, C., & Bushman, B. J. (2016). RETRACTED: Effects of violent media on verbal task performance in gifted and general cohort children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 60(4), 279–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Cooper, J. M., & Hutchinson, D. (1997). Plato complete works. Indianapolis: Hackett.Google Scholar
  24. Corriveau, K. H., Kim, A. L., Schwalen, C. E., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Abraham Lincoln and Harry potter: Children’s differentiation between historical and fantasy characters. Cognition, 113(2), 213–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Cunningham, S., Engelstätter, B., & Ward, M. R. (2016). Violent video games and violent crime. Southern Economic Journal, 82(4), 1247–1265.Google Scholar
  26. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. DeWall, C. N., Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2011). The general aggression model: Theoretical extensions to violence. Psychology of Violence, 1(3), 245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Elson, M. (2016). FlexibleMeasures.com: Competitive reaction time task. Retrieved from http://crtt.flexiblemeasures.com/
  29. Elson, M., Mohseni, M. R., Breuer, J., Scharkow, M., & Quandt, T. (2014). Press CRTT to measure aggressive behavior: The unstandardized use of the competitive reaction time task in aggression research. Psychological Assessment, 26(2), 419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Engelhardt, C. R., Bartholow, B. D., & Saults, J. S. (2011). Violent and nonviolent video games differentially affect physical aggression for individuals high vs. low in dispositional anger. Aggressive Behavior, 37(6), 539–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Engelhardt, C. R., Mazurek, M. O., Hilgard, J., Rouder, J. N., & Bartholow, B. D. (2015). Effects of violent-video-game exposure on aggressive behavior, aggressive-thought accessibility, and aggressive affect among adults with and without autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Science, 26(8), 1187–1200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ferguson, C. J. (2007a). Evidence for publication bias in video game violence effects literature: A meta-analytic review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 12(4), 470–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Ferguson, C. J. (2007b). The good, the bad and the ugly: A meta-analytic review of positive and negative effects of violent video games. Psychiatric Quarterly, 78(4), 309–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Ferguson, C. J. (2015). Do angry birds make for angry children? A meta-analysis of video game influences on children’s and adolescents’ aggression, mental health, prosocial behavior, and academic performance. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(5), 646–666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Ferguson, C. J., Barr, H., Figueroa, G., Foley, K., Gallimore, A., LaQuea, R., … Spanogle, C. (2015). Digital poison? Three studies examining the influence of violent video games on youth. Computers in Human Behavior, 50, 399–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ferguson, C. J., & Donnellan, M. B. (2017). Are associations between “sexist” video games and decreased empathy toward women robust? A reanalysis of Gabbiadini et al. 2016. Journal of youth and adolescence, 46, 1–14.Google Scholar
  37. Ferguson, C. J., & Dyck, D. (2012). Paradigm change in aggression research: The time has come to retire the general aggression model. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17(3), 220–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ferguson, C. J., Garza, A., Jerabeck, J., Ramos, R., & Galindo, M. (2013). Not worth the fuss after all? Cross-sectional and prospective data on violent video game influences on aggression, visuospatial cognition and mathematics ability in a sample of youth. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(1), 109–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ferguson, C. J., & Heene, M. (2012). A vast graveyard of undead theories: Publication bias and psychological science’s aversion to the null. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 555–561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Ferguson, C. J., Ivory, J. D., & Beaver, K. M. (2013). Genetic, maternal, school, intelligence, and media use predictors of adult criminality: A longitudinal test of the catalyst model in adolescence through early adulthood. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 22, 447–460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Ferguson, C. J., & Kilburn, J. (2010). Much ado about nothing: The misestimation and overinterpretation of violent video game effects in eastern and western nations: Comment on Anderson et al.(2010). Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 174–178.Google Scholar
  42. Ferguson, C. J., & Olson, C. K. (2013). Friends, fun, frustration and fantasy: Child motivations for video game play. Motivation and Emotion, 37(1), 154–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Ferguson, C. J., & Olson, C. K. (2014). Video game violence use among “vulnerable” populations: The impact of violent games on delinquency and bullying among children with clinically elevated depression or attention deficit symptoms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43, 127–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Ferguson, C. J., & Rueda, S. M. (2010). The Hitman study: Violent video game exposure effects on aggressive behavior, hostile feelings, and depression. European Psychologist, 15(2), 99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Ferguson, C. J., San Miguel, C., Garza, A., & Jerabeck, J. M. (2012). A longitudinal test of video game violence influences on dating and aggression: A 3-year longitudinal study of adolescents. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 46(2), 141–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Ferguson, C. J., Smith, S., Miller-Stratton, H., Fritz, S., & Heinrich, E. (2008). Aggression in the laboratory: Problems with the validity of the modified Taylor competitive reaction time test as a measure of aggression in media violence studies. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 17(1), 118–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Fischer, P., Kastenmüller, A., & Greitemeyer, T. (2010). Media violence and the self: The impact of personalized gaming characters in aggressive video games on aggressive behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 192–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. France, L. R. (April 20, 2009). “Columbine left its indelible mark on pop culture”. CNN. Retrieved 20 Dec 2017. http://edition.cnn.com/2009/SHOWBIZ/04/20/columbine.pop.culture/index.html?iref=allsearch
  49. Freedman, J. (2002). Media violence and its effect on aggression: Assessing the scientific evidence. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Furedi, F. (2016). Moral panic and reading: Early elite anxieties about the media effect. Cultural Sociology, 10(4), 523–537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Furuya-Kanamori, L., & Doi, S. A. (2016). Angry birds, angry children, and angry meta-analysts: A reanalysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(3), 408–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Gabbiadini, A., Riva, P., Andrighetto, L., Volpato, C., & Bushman, B. J. (2016). Acting like a tough guy: Violent-sexist video games, identification with game characters, masculine beliefs, & empathy for female violence victims. PLoS One, 11(4), e0152121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Gauntlett, D. (2005). Moving experiences: Understanding television’s influences and effects. Lutton: John Libbey.Google Scholar
  54. Gentile, D. A. (2009). Pathological video-game use among youth ages 8 to 18: A national study. Psychological Science, 20(5), 594–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Gentile, D. A., Lynch, P. J., Linder, J. R., & Walsh, D. A. (2004). The effects of violent video game habits on adolescent hostility, aggressive behaviors, and school performance. Journal of Adolescence, 27(1), 5–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Giumetti, G. W., & Markey, P. M. (2007). Violent video games and anger as predictors of aggression. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(6), 1234–1243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Guidelines for the Classification of Computer Games 2012 (Austl.). Retrieved 20 Dec 2017. https://www.legislation.gov.au/Details/F2012L01934
  58. Happ, C., Melzer, A., & Steffgen, G. (2013). Superman vs. BAD man? The effects of empathy and game character in violent video games. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16(10), 774–778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Hilgard, J., Engelhardt, C. R., & Rouder, J. N. (2017). Overstated evidence for short-term effects of violent games on affect and behavior: A reanalysis of Anderson et al.(2010). Psychological Bulletin, 143, 757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Huesmann, L. R. (2010). Nailing the coffin shut on doubts that violent video games stimulate aggression: Comment on Anderson et al.(2010). Psychological Bulletin, 136, 179–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. John, L. K., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2012). Measuring the prevalence of questionable research practices with incentives for truth telling. Psychological Science, 23(5), 524–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. King, D., & Delfabbro, P. (2010). Should Australia have an R 18+ classification for video games? Youth Studies Australia, 29(1), 9.Google Scholar
  63. Markey, P. M., Markey, C. N., & French, J. E. (2015). Violent video games and real-world violence: Rhetoric versus data. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 4(4), 227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Markey, P. M., & Scherer, K. (2009). An examination of psychoticism and motion capture controls as moderators of the effects of violent video games. Computers in Human Behavior, 25(2), 407–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Olson, C. K. (2010). Children's motivations for video game play in the context of normal development. Review of General Psychology, 14(2), 180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Przybylski, A. K., Deci, E. L., Rigby, C. S., & Ryan, R. M. (2014). Competence-impeding electronic games and players’ aggressive feelings, thoughts, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(3), 441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Przybylski, A. K., Ryan, R. M., & Rigby, C. S. (2009). The motivating role of violence in video games. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(2), 243–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Ramirez, J. M., & Andreu, J. M. (2006). Aggression, and some related psychological constructs (anger, hostility, and impulsivity) some comments from a research project. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(3), 276–291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Reiss, A. J., & Roth, J. A. (1993). Understanding and preventing violence: Panel on the understanding and controlof violence behavior. Washington, D.C: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
  70. Rieger, D., Frischlich, L., Wulf, T., Bente, G., & Kneer, J. (2015). Eating ghosts: The underlying mechanisms of mood repair via interactive and noninteractive media. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 4(2), 138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and Well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Ryan, R. M., Rigby, C. S., & Przybylski, A. (2006). The motivational pull of video games: A self-determination theory approach. Motivation and Emotion, 30(4), 344–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Sauer, J. D., Drummond, A., & Nova, N. (2015). Violent video games: The effects of narrative context and reward structure on in-game and postgame aggression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 21(3), 205–214.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Savage, J. (2004). Does viewing violent media really cause criminal violence? A methodological review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10, 99–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Schooler, J. W. (2014). Metascience could rescue the ‘replication crisis’: Independent replication of studies before publication may reveal sources of unreliable results. Nature, 515(7525), 9–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1359–1366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Vokey, J. R., & Read, J. D. (1985). Subliminal messages: Between the devil and the media. American Psychologist, 40(11), 1231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. von Salisch, M., Vogelgesang, J., Kristen, A., & Oppl, C. (2011). Preference for violent electronic games and aggressive behavior among children: The beginning of the downward spiral? Media Psychology, 14(3), 233–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Wetzels, R., Matzke, D., Lee, M. D., Rouder, J. N., Iverson, G. J., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2011). Statistical evidence in experimental psychology: An empirical comparison using 855 t tests. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(3), 291–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Whitaker, J. L., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). RETRACTED:“boom, headshot!” effect of video game play and controller type on firing aim and accuracy. Communication Research, 41(7), 879–891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Willoughby, T., Adachi, P. J., & Good, M. (2012). A longitudinal study of the association between violent video game play and aggression among adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Woolley, J. D., & Van Reet, J. (2006). Effects of context on judgments concerning the reality status of novel entities. Child Development, 77(6), 1778–1793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Aaron Drummond
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • James D. Sauer
    • 2
    • 3
  • Shaun S. Garea
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.School of PsychologyMassey UniversityManawatuNew Zealand
  2. 2.The International Media Laboratory, Massey UniversityManawatuAustralia
  3. 3.Psychology, School of MedicineUniversity of TasmaniaHobartAustralia

Personalised recommendations