Advertisement

Emulation, (Over)imitation and Social Creation of Cultural Information

  • Laura Desirèe Di Paolo
  • Fabio Di Vincenzo
Chapter
Part of the Interdisciplinary Evolution Research book series (IDER)

Abstract

The creation of cultural information by humans is an ability that requires to compound together different factors. Although information needs to be transmitted faithfully enough so to prevent errors, space must be left to create innovations at the same time.

Individual trial and error is the principal source of innovations among all primate species, especially in emulative contexts, but it does not explain the quantity, quality or rapidity of human cultural production. On the other hand, imitation and (over)imitation explain quite well faithful transmission and error control but do not explain the creation of cultural novelties nor the ratchet effect of human culture. To explain these latter components, we need a combination of trial and error in emulative contexts and (over)imitation. Here we suggest that this combination of the ability in creating innovations and transmitting them faithfully occurred for the first time during the Palaeolithic. In that time frame, we can detect the establishment of imitation as the main social learning strategy in the genus Homo. Adopting a niche construction (henceforth NC) paradigm, we propose that this combination became a social characteristic of Homo sapiens which ontogenetically happens when children reach the school age in modern humans.

Keywords

Innovation Monkey Ape Human Hunter-gatherer Palaeolithic Human evolution 

References

  1. Aiello LC, Wheeler P (1995) The expensive-tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Curr Anthropol 36(2):199–221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andersson C (2011) Paleolithic punctuations and equilibria: did retention rather than invention limit technological evolution? PaleoAnthropology 2011:243–259Google Scholar
  3. Antón SC, Potts R, Aiello LC (2014) Evolution of early Homo: an integrated biological perspective. Science 345(6192):1236828CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Asfaw B, Beyene Y, Suwa G, Walter RC, White TD, WoldeGabriel G, Yemane T (1991) The earliest Acheulean from Konso-Gardula. Nature 360(6406):732–735CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bates LA, Byrne RW (2015) Primate social cognition: what we have learned from nonhuman primates and other animals. In: Mikulincer M, Shaver PR (eds) APA handbook of personality and social psychology: vol. 1 attitudes and social cognition. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp 47–78Google Scholar
  6. Berl R, Hewlett B (2015) Cultural variation in the use of overimitation by the Aka and Ngandu of the Congo Basin. PLoS One 10:e0120180CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Beyene Y, Katoh S, WoldeGabriel G, Hart WK, Uto K, Sudo M, Kondo M, Hyodo M, Renne PR, Suwa G (2013) The characteristics and chronology of the earliest Acheulean at Konso, Ethiopia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110(5):1584–1591CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Boëda E (1994) The Levallois concept: variability of methods (Monographie du CRA, 9). CNRS Editions, ParisGoogle Scholar
  9. Boëda E, Geneste J-M, Meignen L (1990) Identification de chaînes opératoires lithiques du Paléolithique ancien et moyen. Paléo 2(1):43–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Boesch C (1997) Three approaches for assessing chimpanzee culture. In: Russon AE, Bard KA, Parker ST (eds) Reaching into thought: the minds of the great apes. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 404–429Google Scholar
  11. Bogin B (1988) Patterns of human growth. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  12. Bogin B (1990) The evolution of human childhood. Bioscience 40:16–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bogin B (1998) Evolutionary and biological aspects of childhood. In: Panter-Brick C (ed) Biosocial perspectives on children. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 10–44Google Scholar
  14. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2005) The origins and evolution of cultures. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  15. Brenna JT, Carlson SE (2014) Docosahexaenoic acid and human brain development: evidence that a dietary supply is needed for optimal development. J Hum Evol 77:99–106CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Bruner E, Iriki A (2016) Extending mind, visuospatial integration, and the evolution of the parietal lobes in the human genus. Quat Int 405(Part A):98–110.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.05.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Bruner E, Manzi G, Arsuaga JL (2003) Encephalization and allometric trajectories in the genus Homo: evidence from the Neandertal and modern lineages. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100(26):15335–15340CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M (2007) Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Dev Sci 10(4):F31–F38CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M (2013) Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, recognize successful actions, but fail to imitate them. Anim Behav 86(4):755–761CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Byrne RW (2002) Imitation of novel complex actions: what does the evidence from animals mean? Adv Study Behav 31:77–105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Byrne RW (2003) Imitation as behaviour parsing. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 358(1431):529–536CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Byrne RW, Russon AE (1998) Learning by imitation: a hierarchical approach. Behav Brain Sci 21(5):667–684PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Caldwell CA (2014) Experimental studies of cumulative culture in modern humans: what are the requirements of the ratchet? In: Mesoudi A, Aoki K (eds) Learning strategies and cultural evolution during the palaeolithic. Springer, Tokyo, pp 145–154Google Scholar
  24. Caldwell CA, Millen AE (2009) Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution is imitation necessary? Psychol Sci 20(12):1478–1483CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Caldwell CA, Schillinger K, Evans CL, Hopper LM (2012) End state copying by humans (Homo sapiens): implications for a comparative perspective on cumulative culture. J Comp Psychol 126(2):161CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Castro L, Toro MA (2004) The evolution of culture: from primate social learning to human culture. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101(27):10235–10240CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Cerling TE, Wynn JG, Andanje SA, Bird MI, Korir DK, Levin NE, Mace W, Macharia AN, Quade J, Remien CH (2011) Woody cover and hominin environments in the past 6 [thinsp] million years. Nature 476(7358):51–56CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Clark A (2013) Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behav Brain Sci 36(3):181–204CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Clark A, Chalmers D (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58(1):7–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Clark A, Chalmers D (2010) Memento’s revenge: the extended mind, extended. In: Menary R (ed) The extended mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MACrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Cordain L, Watkins BA, Mann NJ (2001) Fatty acid composition and energy density of foods available to African hominids. Evolutionary implications for human brain development. World Rev Nutr Diet 90:144–161CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Cunnane SC, Harbige LS, Crawford MA (1993) The importance of energy and nutrient supply in human brain evolution. Nutr Health 9(3):219–235CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Dennell R (2009) DISCUSSION 2: Transitions: behavioral change in the early pleistocene. In: Sourcebook of Paleolithic transitions. Springer, New York, pp 229–234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Di Vincenzo F (2011) Toward a neuro-archaeology of the faculty of language. In: Evoluzione, mente e linguaggio Atti del IV Convegno del CODISCO (2010), Roma, pp 255–266Google Scholar
  35. Di Vincenzo F, Manzi G (2012) Social learning and origin of the language faculty by means of natural selection. J Anthropol Sci 91:23–52Google Scholar
  36. Dominguez-Rodrigo M, Pickering TR, Semaw S, Rogers MJ (2005) Cutmarked bones from Pliocene archaeological sites at Gona, Afar, Ethiopia: implications for the function of the world’s oldest stone tools. J Hum Evol 48(2):109–121CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Dunbar RIM (1998) The social brain hypothesis. Evol Anthropol 6:178–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Dunbar RIM (2003) The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annu Rev Anthropol 32:163–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Fernández MH, Vrba ES (2006) Plio-Pleistocene climatic change in the Turkana Basin (East Africa): evidence from large mammal faunas. J Hum Evol 50(6):595–626CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Fragaszy D (2012) Community resources for learning: how capuchin monkeys construct technical traditions. Biol Theory 6(3):231–240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Goren-Inbar N, Sharon G, Alperson-Afil N, Laschiver I (2008) The Acheulean massive scrapers of Gesher Benot Ya’aqov—a product of the biface chaîne opératoire. J Hum Evol 55(4):702–712CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Harris PL (2012) Trusting what you’re told: how children learn from others. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MACrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Hobaiter C, Byrne RW (2010) Able-bodied wild chimpanzees imitate a motor procedure used by a disabled individual to overcome handicap. PLoS One 5(8):e11959CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. Högberg A, Gärdenfors P, Larsson L (2015) Knowing, learning and teaching – how Homo became docens. Camb Archaeol J 25(4):847–858CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Hopper LM, Marshall-Pescini S, Whiten A (2012) Social learning and culture in child and chimpanzee. In: de Waal FB, Ferrari PF (eds) The primate mind: built to connect with other minds. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 99–118Google Scholar
  46. Horner V, Whiten A (2005) Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Anim Cogn 8(3):164–181CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Jeffares B (2009) The evolution of technical competence: strategic and economic thinking. In: ASCS09: Proceedings of the 9th Conference of the Australasian Society for Cognitive Science, pp 162–169Google Scholar
  48. Jeffares B (2010) The co-evolution of tools and minds: cognition and material culture in the hominin lineage. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 9(4):503–520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Johnson-Laird PN (1988) The computer and the mind: an introduction to cognitive science. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  50. Kenward B (2012) Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. J Exp Child Psychol 112:195–207CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Kenward B, Karlsson M, Persson J (2011) Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 278:1239–1246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Koops K, Visalberghi E, van Schaik CP (2014) The ecology of primate material culture. Biol Lett 10(11):20140508CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Kyriacou A, Bruner E (2011) Special issue: innovation and the evolution of human behavior brain evolution, innovation, and endocranial variations in fossil hominids. PaleoAnthropology 130:143Google Scholar
  54. Laland KN, O’Brien MJ (2011) Cultural niche construction: an introduction. Biol Theory 6(3):191–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Legare CH, Nielsen M (2015) Imitation and innovation: the dual engines of cultural learning. Trends Cogn Sci 19(11):688–699CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Liu C, Teng Z-Q, Santistevan NJ, Szulwach KE, Guo W, Jin P, Zhao X (2010) Epigenetic regulation of miR-184 by MBD1 governs neural stem cell proliferation and differentiation. Cell Stem Cell 6(5):433–444CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Lyons DE, Young AG, Keil FC (2007) The hidden structure of overimitation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104(50):19751–19756CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Lyons DE, Damrosch DH, Lin JK, Macris DM, Keil FC (2011) The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366(1567):1158–1167CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  59. Maslin MA, Shultz S, Trauth MH (2015) A synthesis of the theories and concepts of early human evolution. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 370(1663):20140064CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  60. McGuigan N, Makinson J, Whiten A (2011) From over-imitation to super-copying: adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. Br J Psychol 102(1):1–18CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Morin O (2016) How traditions live and die. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  62. Nielsen M (2006) Copying actions and copying outcomes: social learning through the second year. Dev psychol 42(3):555–565CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Nielsen M (2011) Imitation, pretend play, and childhood: essential elements in the evolution of human culture. J Comp Psychol 126(2):170CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Nielsen M, Tomaselli K (2010) Over-imitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychol Sci 21:729–736CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Nielsen M, Moore C, Mohamedally J (2012) Young children overimitate in third-party contexts. J Exp Child Psychol 112:73–83CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Odling-Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman MW (2003) Niche construction: the neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  67. Plummer TW, Ditchfield PW, Bishop LC, Kingston JD, Ferraro JV, Braun DR, Hertel F, Potts R (2009) Oldest evidence of toolmaking hominins in a grassland-dominated ecosystem. PLoS One 4(9):e7199CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  68. Pontzer H, Brown MH, Raichlen DA, Dunsworth H, Hare B, Walker K, Luke A, Dugas LR, Durazo-Arvizu R, Schoeller D, Plange-Rhule J, Bovet P, Forrester TE, Lambert EV, Thompson ME, Shumaker RW, Ross SR (2016) Metabolic acceleration and the evolution of human brain size and life history. Nature 533(7603):390–392CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  69. Potts R (2012) Environmental and behavioral evidence pertaining to the evolution of early Homo. Curr Anthropol 53(S6):S299–S317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Pradhan GR, Tennie C, van Schaik CP (2012) Social organization and the evolution of cumulative technology in apes and hominins. J Hum Evol 63(1):180–190CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Proffitt T, Luncz LV, Falotico T, Ottoni EB, de la Torre I, Haslam M (2016) Wild monkeys flake stone tools. Nature 539(7627):85–88CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Reader SM, Laland KN (2001) Primate innovation: sex, age and social rank differences. Int J Primatol 22(5):787–805CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Reader SM, Laland KN (2002) Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci 99(7):4436–4441CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Rogoff B (1990) Apprenticeship in thinking: cognitive development in social context. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  75. Russon AE, Galdikas BM (1993) Imitation in free-ranging rehabilitant orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 107(2):147CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Semaw S, Renne P, Harris JWK, Feibel CS, Bernor RL, Fesseha N, Mowbray K (1997) 2.5-million-year-old stone tools from Gona, Ethiopia. Nature 385(6614):333–336CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2015) Social cognition. Anim Behav 103:191–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Shimpi PM, Akhtar N, Moore C (2013) Toddlers’ imitative learning in interactive and observational contexts: the role of age and familiarity of the model. J Exp Child Psychol 116(2):309–323CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Shipton C (2010) Imitation and shared intentionality in the Acheulean. Camb Archaeol J 20(2):197–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Shipton C, Nielsen M (2015) Before cumulative culture. Hum Nat 26(3):331–345CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Shipton C, Nielsen M (2018) The acquisition of biface knapping skill in the Acheulean. In: Di Paolo LD, Di Vincenzo F, De Petrillo F (eds) Evolution of primate social cognition. Springer, ChamGoogle Scholar
  82. Shipton C, Petraglia MD, Paddayya K (2009) Inferring aspects of Acheulean sociality and cognition from lithic technology. In: Adams B, Blades BS (eds) Lithic materials and palaeolithic societies. Wiley, Chichester, pp 219–231Google Scholar
  83. Somel M, Liu X, Khaitovich P (2014) Human brain evolution: transcripts, metabolites and their regulators. Nat Rev Neurosci 14(2):112–127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Sponheimer M, Alemseged Z, Cerling TE, Grine FE, Kimbel WH, Leakey MG, Lee-Thorp JA, Kyalo Manthi F, Reed KE, Wood BA, Wynn JG (2013) Isotopic evidence of early hominin diets. Proc Natl Acad Sci 110(26):10513–10518CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Sterelny K (2011) From hominins to humans: how sapiens became behaviourally modern. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366(1566):809–822CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  86. Sterelny K (2012) The evolved apprentice. MIT Press, Cambridge, MACrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Stout D, Chaminade T (2012) Stone tools, language and the brain in human evolution. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 367(1585):75–87CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  88. Stout D, Apel J, Commander J, Roberts M (2014) Late Acheulean technology and cognition at Boxgrove, UK. J Archaeol Sci 41:576–590CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2012) Untrained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) fail to imitate novel actions. PLoS One 7(8):e41548CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  90. Tennie C, Walter V, Gampe A, Carpenter M, Tomasello M (2014) Limitations to the cultural ratchet effect in young children. J Exp Child Psychol 126:152–160CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Texier PJ, Roche H (1995) The impact of predetermination on the development of some Acheulean chaînes opératoires. Evolución Humana en Europa y los Yacimientos de la. Sierra de Atapuerca 2:403–420Google Scholar
  92. Tomasello M (1996) Do apes ape? In: Heyes CM, Galef BG (eds) Social learning animals: the roots of culture. Academic, London, pp 319–346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  94. Villmoare B, Kimbel WH, Seyoum C, Campisano CJ, DiMaggio EN, Rowan J, Braun DR, Arrowsmith JR, Reed KE (2015) Early Homo at 2.8 Ma from Ledi-Geraru, Afar, Ethiopia. Science 347(6228):1352–1355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Visalberghi E, Fragaszy D (2012) Learning how to forage: socially biased individual learning and “niche construction” in wild capuchin monkeys. In: de Waal F, Ferrari PF (eds) The priemate mind. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 81–98Google Scholar
  96. Vrba ES (1995) Paleoclimate and evolution, with emphasis on human origins. Yale University Press, New HavenGoogle Scholar
  97. Whiten A, Horner V, Litchfield CA, Marshall-Pescini S (2004) How do apes ape? Learn Behav 32(1):36–52CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Whiten A, Horner V, De Waal FBM (2005) Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 437(7059):737–740CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Whiten A, McGuigan N, Marshall-Pescini S, Hopper LM (2009) Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364(1528):2417–2428CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  100. Winton V (2005) An investigation of knapping-skill development in the manufacture of palaeolithic handaxes. In: Roux V, Brill B (eds) Stone knapping: the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp 109–116Google Scholar
  101. Wood B, Strait D (2004) Patterns of resource use in early Homo and Paranthropus. J Hum Evol 46(2):119–162CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Wood LA, Kendal RL, Flynn EG (2012) Context-dependent model-based biases in cultural transmission: children’s imitation is affected by model age over model knowledge state. Evol Hum Behav 33(4):387–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Wood LA, Kendal RL, Flynn EG (2013a) Copy me or copy you? The effect of prior experience on social learning. Cognition 127(2):203–213CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Wood LA, Kendal RL, Flynn EG (2013b) Whom do children copy? Model-based biases in social learning. Dev Rev 33(4):341–356CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Zink KD, Lieberman DE (2016) Impact of meat and Lower Palaeolithic food processing techniques on chewing in humans. Nature 531(7595):500–503CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  106. Zollikofer CPE, de León MSP (2013) Pandora’s growing box: inferring the evolution and development of hominin brains from endocasts. Evol Anthropol 22(1):20–33CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  107. Zwirner E, Thornton A (2015) Cognitive requirements of cumulative culture: teaching is useful but not essential. Sci Rep 5:16781CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Laura Desirèe Di Paolo
    • 1
  • Fabio Di Vincenzo
    • 2
  1. 1.Lichtenberg-Kolleg—Institute for Advanced Study at the Georg-August-Universität GöttingenGöttingenGermany
  2. 2.Department of Environmental BiologySapienza Università di RomaRomaItaly

Personalised recommendations