Skip to main content

Situating Interaction in Peripersonal and Extrapersonal Space: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Contributions To Phenomenology ((CTPH,volume 95))

Abstract

In this chapter I focus on the relationship between embodied intersubjective interactions and the kind of spaces that shape and are shaped by such interactions. After clarifying some of the theoretical background involved in questions about social cognition, I review several empirical studies that suggest that social interactions and social relations can change our perceptions of the reachable (peripersonal) space around us, as well as the more distant (extrapersonal) space beyond our immediate reach. These perceptions operate within the framework of material culture and impact our experience of space as it is organized by cultural artifacts and practices. In this respect, the analysis provided by Material Engagement Theory (Malafouris L, How things shape the mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2013) helps us understand the role of material arrangements as they define affordances for action and interaction, correlated to transformations from individual body-schematic processes to intercorporeal processes in joint action. These same processes can be carried over into discussions of space and place as experienced on the larger stages of social-cultural activities.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Soliman and Glenberg (2014) used a complicated technique called the ‘flash/buzz’ paradigm (Maravita et al. 2002) to indirectly measure to what degree peripersonal space is expanded. A buzzer is attached to the index finger and another attached to the thumb. One of the buzzers is activated, and the task is to indicate (using a foot pedal) whether the thumb or index finger had been stimulated. Two LED lights are located next to the 2 fingers, respectively. LEDs flash in temporal synchrony with the buzzer but spatially incongruously (e.g. the thumb is stimulated by the buzzer but the LED next to the index finger is flashed). In these incongruous trials, reaction times are slowed in reporting which finger is buzzed, and participants make errors. Maravita et al. (2002) show that tool use can modify the interference on the incongruous trials. Before using a tool, the LEDs interfere with localization of the vibration only when they are near the fingers, that is, in peripersonal space. After using a tool (e.g., a rake), the LEDs can be located at the end of the tool, outside of the usual peripersonal space, and still interfere with localization of the active buzzer. This is taken as a measure of how much the body schema has incorporated the tool (or how much peripersonal space has expanded).

References

  • Adams, G., S.L. Anderson, and J.K. Adonu. 2004. The cultural grounding of closeness and intimacy. In Handbook of Closeness and Intimacy, ed. D.J. Mashek and A. Aron, 321–339. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baird, J.A., and D.A. Baldwin. 2001. Making sense of human behavior: Action parsing and intentional inference. In Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition, ed. B.F. Malle, L.J. Moses, and D.A. Baldwin, 193–206. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D.A., and J.A. Baird. 2001. Discerning intentions in dynamic human action. Trends in Cognitive Science 5 (4): 171–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becchio, C., V. Manera, L. Sartori, A. Cavallo, and U. Castiello. 2012. Grasping intentions: From thought experiments to empirical evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6: 117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi, G., and S. Aglioti. 1997. The body in the brain: Neural bases of corporeal awareness. Trends in Neuroscience 20: 560–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2010. The body in the brain revisited. Experimental Brain Research 200: 25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berti, A., and F. Frassinetti. 2000. When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12 (3): 415–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonini, L., M. Maranesi, A. Livi, L. Fogassi, and G. Rizzolatti. 2014. Space-dependent representation of objects and other’s action in monkey ventral premotor grasping neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience 34 (11): 4108–4119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brincker, M. 2014. Navigating beyond “here & now” affordances – On sensorimotor maturation and “false belief” performance. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1433. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caggiano, V., L. Fogassi, G. Rizzolatti, P. Thier, and A. Casile. 2009. Mirror neurons differentially encode the peripersonal and extrapersonal space of monkeys. Science 324: 403–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catmur, C., V. Walsh, and C. Heyes. 2007. Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Current Biology 17: 1527–1531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coulon, M., C. Hemimou, and A. Streri. 2013. Effects of seeing and hearing vowels on neonatal facial imitation. Infancy 18 (5): 782–796.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G. 2005. Mirror neurons and action observation. Is simulation involved? ESF Interdisciplines. http://www.interdisciplines.org/mirror/papers/.

  • De Jaegher, H., E.A. Di Paolo, and S. Gallagher. 2010. Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (10): 441–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinstein, I., C. Thomas, M. Behrmann, and D.J. Heeger. 2008. A mirror up to nature. Current Biology 18 (1): R13–R18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farnè, A., A. Iriki, and E. Làdavas. 2005. Shaping multisensory action-space with tools: Evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia 43: 238–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. 2006. The intrinsic spatial frame of reference. In The Blackwell Companion to Phenomenology and Existentialism, ed. H. Dreyfus and M. Wrathall, 346–355. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2013. The socially extended mind. Cognitive Systems Research 25–26: 4–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2015. Doing the math: Calculating the role of evolution and enculturation in the origins of mathematical reasoning. Progress in Biophysics and Molecular Biology 119: 341–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., and T. Ransom. 2016. Artifacting minds: Material engagement theory and joint action. In Embodiment in Evolution and Culture, ed. C. Tewes, 337–351. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., L. Reinerman, B. Janz, P. Bockelman, and J. Trempler. 2015. A Neurophenomenology of Awe and Wonder: Towards a Non-reductionist Cognitive Science. London: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., S. Martínez Muñoz, and M. Gastelum. 2017. Action-space and time: Towards an enactive hermeneutics. In Hermeneutics: place and space, ed. B. Janz, 83–96. Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J.J. 1977. The theory of affordances. In Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Toward an Ecological Psychology, ed. R. Shaw and J. Bransford, 67–82. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A.I. 2014. The bodily formats approach to embodied cognition. In Current Controversies in Philosophy of Mind, ed. U. Kriegel, 91–108. New York/London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A.I., and F. de Vignemont. 2009. Is social cognition embodied? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13 (4): 154–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gosden, C. 2008. Social ontologies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B 363: 2003–2010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heed, T., B. Habets, N. Sebanz, and G. Knoblich. 2010. Others’ actions reduce crossmodal integration in peripersonal space. Current Biology 20 (15): 1345–1349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. 1989. Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy: Second Book, Studies in the Phenomenology of Constitution. Trans. R. Rojcewicz and A. Schuwer. Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jerry, R.H. 2004. A brief exploration of space: Some observations on law school architecture. University of Toledo Law Review 36: 85–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. 2015. The embodied meaning of architecture. In Mind in Architecture: Neuroscience, Embodiment, and the Future of Design, ed. S. Robinson, J. Pallasmaa, and J., 33–50. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kant, I. 1992. Concerning the ultimate ground of the differentiation of directions in space. In The Cambridge Edition of the Works of Immanuel Kant. Theoretical Philosophy, 1755–1770, ed. D. Walford and R. Meerbote, 365–372. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keven, N., and K.A. Akins. 2016, July. Neonatal imitation in context: Sensory-motor development in the perinatal period. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14: 1–107. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X16000911.

  • Kitayama, S., and J. Park. 2010. Cultural neuroscience of the self: Understanding the social grounding of the brain. Social Cognitive Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 111–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. 2013. How Things Shape the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., and A. Iriki. 2004. Tools for the body (schema). Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8 (2): 79–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., C. Spence, S. Kennett, and J. Driver. 2002. Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans. Cognition 83 (2): B25–B34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A., and M.K. Moore. 1977. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, New Series 198 (4312): 75–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R., ed. 2010. The Extended Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. 1968. The Visible and the Invisible: Followed by Working Notes. Trans. A. Lingis. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2012. Phenomenology of Perception. Trans: D.A. Landes. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, D. 2010. The enigma of reversibility and the genesis of sense in Merleau-Ponty. Continental Philosophy Review 43 (2): 141–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R.E., and Y. Miyamoto. 2005. The influence of culture: Holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (10): 467–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oostenbroek, J., T. Suddendorf, M. Nielsen, J. Redshaw, S. Kennedy-Costantini, J. Davis, and V. Slaughter. 2016. Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology 26 (10): 1334–1338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pasqualini, I., J. Llobera, and O. Blanke. 2013. “Seeing” and “feeling” architecture: How bodily self-consciousness alters architectonic experience and affects the perception of interiors. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pérez-Gómez, A. 1998. The case for hermeneutics as architectural discourse. In Architecture and Teaching-Epistemological Foundations. 31st EAAE Workshop (European Association for Architectural Education), ed. H. Dunin-Woyseth and H., 21–29. Paris: Comportements.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramstead, M.J.D., S.P. Veissière, and L.J. Kirmayer. 2016. Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attention. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1090.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. 2008. How Infants Know Minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, E., and J. Kiverstein. 2014. A rich landscape of affordances. Ecological Psychology 26 (4): 325–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., M.H. Johnson, and G. Csibra. 2006. The development and neural basis of referential gaze perception. Social Neuroscience 1 (3–4): 220–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soliman, T., and A.M. Glenberg. 2014. The embodiment of culture. In The Routledge Handbook of Embodied Cognition, ed. L. Shapiro, 207–219. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soliman, T., A. Gibson, and A.M. Glenberg. 2013. Sensory motor mechanisms unify psychology: The embodiment of culture. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soliman, T.M., R. Ferguson, M.S. Dexheimer, and A.M. Glenberg. 2015. Consequences of joint action: Entanglement with your partner. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 144 (4): 873–888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, L.H. 2013. Kindergarten chats and other writings. Toronto: Reed Books, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Temple, P. 2014. The Physical University: Contours of Space and Place in Higher Education. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Teneggi, C., E. Canzoneri, G. di Pellegrino, and A. Serino. 2013. Social modulation of peripersonal space boundaries. Current Biology 23: 406–411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. 1979. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before Speech, ed. M. Bullowa, 321–347. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., and P. Hubley. 1978. Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In Action, Gesture and Symbol: The Emergence of Language, ed. A. Lock, 183–229. London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Elk, M. 2014. The effect of manipulability and religion on the multisensory integration of objects in peripersonal space. Cognitive Neuroscience 5 (1): 36–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vincini, S., J. Yuna, E.H. Buder, and S. Gallagher. 2017. Neonatal imitation: Theory, experimental design and significance for the field of social cognition. Frontiers in Psychology – Cognitive Science 8: 1323. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, A.L., and J.A. Sommerville. 2000. Twelve-month-old infants interpret action in context. Psychological Science 11 (1): 73–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I presented an earlier version of this paper as a keynote lecture at the ICSC 2015: 6th International Conference on Spatial Cognition. Rome 7–11 September 2015. My research for this project has been supported by the Humboldt Foundation’s Anneliese Maier Research Award, and as a Senior Visiting Researcher at Keble College, Oxford in 2016.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shaun Gallagher .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Gallagher, S. (2018). Situating Interaction in Peripersonal and Extrapersonal Space: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives. In: Hünefeldt, T., Schlitte, A. (eds) Situatedness and Place. Contributions To Phenomenology, vol 95. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-92937-8_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics