Skip to main content

Boredom: What Is It Good For?

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Function of Emotions

Abstract

Boredom is an ubiquitous and consequential human emotion. This chapter argues that it functions as a self-regulatory signal indicating that our cognitive resources are not engaged. It provides a definition of state boredom before developing the broad notion that trait boredom represents a chronic disposition toward maladaptively responding to the boredom signal (i.e., state boredom). The chapter reviews the nascent research employing functional neuroimaging to understand boredom and casts it as being mired in the “here and now” with no clear avenues for escape. Next, it outlines a specific hypothesis that trait boredom arises in circumstances of regulatory non-fit – when our preferred mode of goal pursuit does not match our current behavior. Finally, the chapter explores the notion that state boredom is not intrinsically good or bad. The signal itself does not evaluate what we are doing in any obvious way but merely indicates that change is needed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 69.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 89.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adaman, J. E., & Blaney, P. H. (1995). The effects of musical mood induction on creativity. The Journal of Creative Behaviour, 29, 95–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, V. V., Anderson, P., Northam, E., Jacobs, R., & Catroppa, C. (2001). Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 385–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2012). The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. The Neuroscientist, 18, 251–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Avnet, T., & Higgins, E. T. (2003). Locomotion, assessment, and regulatory fit: Value transfer from “how” to “what.”. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 525–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barmack, J. E. (1939). Studies on the psychophysiology of boredom: Part I. The effect of 15 mgs. Of benzedrine sulfate and 60 mgs. Of ephedrine hydrochloride on blood pressure, report of boredom and other factors. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 494–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Self-regulation and the executive function of the self. In Handbook of self and identity (Vol. 1, pp. 197–217). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bench, S. W., & Lench, H. C. (2013). On the function of boredom. Behavioral Sciences, 3, 459–472.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczynski, A., McConaghy, N., & Frankova, A. (1990). Boredom proneness in pathological gambling. Psychological Reports, 67, 35–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charnov, E. L. (1976). Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 9, 129–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 8719–8724.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel-now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 59–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (1992). Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dal Mas, D. E., & Wittmann, B. C. (2017). Avoiding boredom: Caudate and insula activity reflects boredom-elicited purchase bias. Cortex, 92, 57–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J. (2013). Descent of the doldrums. Scientific American Mind, 24, 54–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J., Hammerschmidt, T., Marty-Duga, J., & Smilek, D. (2017). Boredom: Under-aroused and restless. Submitted to Consciousness & Cognition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J., & Isacescu, J. (2017). The bored brain: Insular cortex and the default mode network. PsyArXiv Preprints. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/AQBCD.

  • Danckert, J., & Merrifield, C. (in press). Boredom, sustained attention and the default mode network. Experimental Brain Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, L. M., Tze, V. M., & Goetz, T. (2015). Examining boredom: Different causes for different coping profiles. Learning and Individual Differences, 37, 255–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daschmann, E. C., Goetz, T., & Stupnisky, R. H. (2011). Testing the predictors of boredom at school: Development and validation of the precursors to boredom scales. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 421–440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dockree, P. M., Bellgrove, M. A., O’Keeffe, F. M., Moloney, P., Aimola, L., Carton, S., & Robertson, I. H. (2006). Sustained attention in traumatic brain injury (TBI) and healthy controls: Enhanced sensitivity with dual-task load. Experimental Brain Research, 168, 218–229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dockree, P. M., Kelly, S. P., Roche, R. A., Hogan, M. J., Reilly, R. B., & Robertson, I. H. (2004). Behavioural and physiological impairments of sustained attention after traumatic brain injury. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 403–414.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Smilek, D. (2012). The unengaged mind defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 482–495.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eccleston, C., & Crombez, G. (1999). Pain demands attention: A cognitive-affective model of the interruptive function of pain. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 356–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, R., Newman, J. L., Longe, O. A., & Deakin, J. W. (2003). Differential response patterns in the striatum and orbitofrontal cortex to financial reward in humans: A parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 303–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elpidorou, A. (2014). The bright side of boredom. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Essed, N. H., van Staveren, W. A., Kok, F. J., Ormel, W., Zeinstra, G., & de Graaf, C. (2006). The effect of repeated exposure to fruit drinks on intake, pleasantness and boredom in young and elderly adults. Physiology & Behaviour, 89, 335–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fahlman, S. A., Mercer, K. B., Gaskovski, P., Eastwood, A. E., & Eastwood, J. D. (2009). Does a lack of life meaning cause boredom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal, and experimental analyses. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 307–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fahlman, S. A., Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., & Eastwood, J. D. (2013). Development and validation of the multidimensional state boredom scale. Assessment, 20, 68–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, R., & Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom proneness-the development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 4–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, F. S., & Schulman, M. (1978). Female self-report and autonomic responses to sexually pleasurable and sexually aversive readings. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 7, 443–453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C. D. (1993). Boredom at work: A neglected concept. Human Relations, 46, 395–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Christoff, K. (2015). The wandering brain: Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. NeuroImage, 111, 611–621.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning: Learning, development, and conceptual change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K., & Middlewood, B. L. (2014). Approaching novel thoughts: Understanding why elation and boredom promote associative thought more than distress and relaxation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 50–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geiwitz, P. J. (1966). Structure of boredom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 592–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Hall, N. C., Nett, U. E., Pekrun, R., & Lipnevich, A. A. (2014). Types of boredom: An experience sampling approach. Motivation and Emotion, 38, 401–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., … Thompson, P. M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 101, 8174–8179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, Y. K., Eastwood, J. D., LaGuardia, J., & Danckert, J. (2011). Boredom: An emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia, or depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30, 647–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez-Ramirez, J., & Costa, T. (2017). Boredom begets creativity: A solution to the exploitation-exploration trade-off in predictive coding. Bio-Systems, 162, 168–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, J. A., O’Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Encoding predictive reward value in human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Science, 301, 1104–1107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenson, R. R. (1953). On boredom. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 1, 7–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 253–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1971). Creativity tests for children: A manual of interpretations. Orange, CA: Sheridan Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (2005). Value from regulatory fit. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 209–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, A. B. (1975). Work variety and individual differences in occupational boredom. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 128–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inzlicht, M., & Legault, L. (2014). No pain, no gain: How distress underlies effective control (and unites diverse psychological phenomena). In J. Forgas & E. Harmon-Jones (Eds.), The control within: Motivation and its regulation (pp. 115–132). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isacescu, J., & Danckert, J. (in press). Exploring the relationship between boredom proneness and self-control in traumatic brain injury (TBI). Experimental Brain Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isacescu, J., Struk, A. A., & Danckert, J. (2016). Cognitive and affective predictors of boredom proneness. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 1741–1748.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iso-Ahola, S. E., & Crowley, E. D. (1991). Adolescent substance abuse and leisure boredom. Journal of Leisure Research, 23, 260–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jang, E. H., Park, B. J., Park, M. S., Kim, S. H., & Sohn, J. H. (2015). Analysis of physiological signals for recognition of boredom, pain, and surprise emotions. Journal of Physiological Anthropology, 34, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, L. D., & O’Malley, P. M. (1986). Why do the nation’s students use drugs and alcohol? Self-reported reasons from nine national surveys. Journal of Drug Issues, 16, 29–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joireman, J., Anderson, J., & Strathman, A. (2003). The aggression paradox: Understanding links among aggression, sensation seeking, and the consideration of future consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1287–1302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kass, S. J., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1990). Boredom proneness: Its relationship to type a behaviours and sensation seeking. Psychology: A Journal of Human Behaviour, 27, 7–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keating, D. P. (2012). Cognitive and brain development in adolescence. Enfance, 64, 267–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenah, K., Bernhardt, J., Cumming, T., Spratt, N., Luker, J. & Janssen, H. (in press). Boredom in patients with acquired brain injuries during rehabilitation: A scoping review. Disability and Rehabilitation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Thompson, E. P., Higgins, E. T., Atash, M., Pierro, A., Shah, J. Y., & Spiegel, S. (2000). To “do the right thing” or to “just do it”: Locomotion and assessment as distinct self-regulatory imperatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 793–815.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 661–679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, J. I. (1959). Psychophysiological approaches to the evaluation of psychotherapeutic process and outcome. In E. A. Rubinstein & M. B. Parloff (Eds.), Research in psychotherapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, J. I., & Lacey, B. C. (1970). Some autonomic-central nervous system interrelationships. In P. Black (Ed.), Physiological correlates of emotion (pp. 205–228). New York, NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R. W. (1990). Emotions and the creative process: Anxiety, boredom, and enjoyment as predictors of creative writing. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 9, 275–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • London, H., Schubert, D. S., & Washburn, D. (1972). Increase of autonomic arousal by boredom. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 80, 29–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, U., Melin, B., Evans, G. W., & Holmberg, L. (1993). Physiological deactivation after two contrasting tasks at a video display terminal: Learning vs repetitive data entry. Ergonomics, 36, 601–611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malkovsky, E., Merrifield, C., Goldberg, Y., & Danckert, J. (2012). Exploring the relationship between boredom and sustained attention. Experimental Brain Research, 221, 59–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, S., & Cadman, R. (2014). Does being bored make us more creative? Creativity Research Journal, 26, 165–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, 393–395.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mednick, M. T., & Mednick, S. A. (1968). The remote associations test. The Journal of Creative Behaviour, 2, 213–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melton, A. M., & Schulenberg, S. E. (2009). A confirmatory factor analysis of the boredom proneness scale. The Journal of Psychology, 143, 493–508.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function, 214, 655–667.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, K. B., & Eastwood, J. D. (2010). Is boredom associated with problem gambling behaviour? It depends on what you mean by ‘boredom’. International Gambling Studies, 10, 91–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., Fahlman, S. A., & Eastwood, J. D. (2011). The measurement of boredom: Differences between existing self-report scales. Assessment, 20, 585–596.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Hunter, J. A., & Eastwood, J. D. (2013). Is trait boredom redundant? Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 32, 897–916.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merrifield, C., & Danckert, J. (2014). Characterizing the psychophysiological signature of boredom. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 481–491.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mikulas, W. L., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1993). The essence of boredom. The Psychological Record, 43, 3–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 638–662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mugon, J., Danckert, J., & Eastwood, J. (in press). The costs and benefits of boredom in the classroom. In The Cambridge handbook on motivation and learning. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mugon, J., Struk, A., & Danckert, J. (in press). A failure to launch: Exploring the self-regulatory profile of boredom. Frontiers in Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., & Daniels, L. M. (2010). What to do when feeling bored?: Students’ strategies for coping with boredom. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 626–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., & Hall, N. C. (2011). Coping with boredom in school: An experience sampling perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 49–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nietzsche, F. (1996). Human, all too human: A book for free spirits. (R.J. Hollingdale, Trans.). Cambridge texts in the history of philosophy. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., & Andrews, C. (2001). Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 95–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keeffe, F. M., Dockree, P. M., Moloney, P., Carton, S., & Robertson, I. H. (2007). Characterising error-awareness of attentional lapses and inhibitory control failures in patients with traumatic brain injury. Experimental Brain Research, 180, 59–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohsuga, M., Shimono, F., & Genno, H. (2001). Assessment of phasic work stress using autonomic indices. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40, 211–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pattyn, N., Neyt, X., Henderickx, D., & Soetens, E. (2008). Psychophysiological investigation of vigilance decrement: Boredom or cognitive fatigue? Physiology & Behavior, 93, 369–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poletti, M. (2009). Adolescent brain development and executive functions: A prefrontal framework for developmental psycho-pathologies. Clinical Neuropsychiatry: Journal of Treatment Evaluation, 6, 155–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rupp, D. E., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1997). The role of boredom proneness in self-reported anger and aggression. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 925–936.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., Weir, C., Harpster, L., & Morgan, C. (1992). Once a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a self-regulatory mechanism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 379–390.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schopenhauer, A. (1995). The world as will and idea. (J. Berman, Trans.). (Book Two Section 29, page 85). London, England: Everyman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B., Ward, A., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., White, K., & Lehman, D. R. (2002). Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1178–1197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seel, R. T., & Kreutzer, J. S. (2003). Depression assessment after traumatic brain injury: An empirically based classification method. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84, 1621–1628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive Science, 20, 605–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sidlauskaite, J., Wiersema, J. R., Roeyers, H., Krebs, R. M., Vassena, E., Fias, W., … Sonuga-Barke, E. (2014). Anticipatory processes in brain state switching—Evidence from a novel cued-switching task implicating default mode and salience networks. NeuroImage, 98, 359–365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smilek, D., Carriere, J. S., & Cheyne, J. A. (2010). Out of mind, out of sight: Eye blinking as indicator and embodiment of mind wandering. Psychological Science, 21, 786–789.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. P. (1981). Boredom: A review. Human Factors, 23, 329–340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sommers, J., & Vodanovich, S. J. (2000). Boredom proneness: Its relationship to psychological-and physical-health symptoms. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56, 149–155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spaeth, M., Weichold, K., & Silbereisen, R. K. (2015). The development of leisure boredom in early adolescence: Predictors and longitudinal associations with delinquency and depression. Developmental Psychology, 51, 1380–1394.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Struk, A., Scholer, A., & Danckert, J. (2015). Perceived control predicts engagement and diminished boredom. Poster presented at the Canadian Society for Brain. Ottawa, Canada: Behaviour and Cognitive Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Struk, A. A., Carriere, J. S., Cheyne, J. A., & Danckert, J. (2017). A short boredom proneness scale: Development and psychometric properties. Assessment, 24, 346–359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Struk, A. A., Mugon, J., Scholer, A. A., Sokolowski, M. B., & Danckert, J. (in preparation). Of fruit flies and men: Self-regulation and the foraging gene (PRKG1) in humans.

    Google Scholar 

  • Struk, A. A., Scholer, A. A., & Danckert, J. (2016). A self-regulatory approach to understanding boredom proneness. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 1388–1401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72, 271–324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thackray, R. I. (1981). The stress of boredom and monotony: A consideration of the evidence. Psychosomatic Medicine, 43, 165–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thackray, R. I., Bailey, J. P., & Touchstone, R. M. (1977). Physiological, subjective, and performance correlates of reported boredom and monotony while performing a simulated radar control task. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance (pp. 203–215). Boston, MA: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Theobold, D. E., Kirsh, K. L., Holtsclaw, E., Donaghy, K., & Passik, S. D. (2003). An open label pilot study of citalopram for depression and boredom in ambulatory cancer patients. Palliative & Supportive Care, 1, 71–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolinski, B., & Di Perna, A. (2016). Play it loud: An epic history of the style, sound, & revolution of the electric guitar. New York, NY: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolstoy, L. N. (1899). Anna Karénina. New York, NY: Thomas Y. Crowell Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Troutwine, R., & O’Neal, E. D. (1981). Volition, performance of a boring task, and time estimation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52, 865–866.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tze, V. M., Klassen, R. M., & Daniels, L. M. (2014). Patterns of boredom and its relationship with perceived autonomy support and engagement. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39, 175–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 55–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, M., Keller, J., & Grön, G. (2016). Neural signatures of experimentally induced flow experiences identified in a typical fMRI block design with BOLD imaging. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 496–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, M., Keller, J., Hoenig, K., Waller, C., & Grön, G. (2014). Neural correlates of experimentally induced flow experiences. NeuroImage, 86, 194–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2011). On boredom: Lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 181–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Tilburg, W. A., Igou, E. R., & Sedikides, C. (2013). In search of meaningfulness: Nostalgia as an antidote to boredom. Emotion, 13, 450–461.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, H. M. (1926). The human factor and industrial accidents. International Labour Review, 13, 673–683.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J. (2003a). Psychometric measures of boredom: A review of the literature. The Journal of Psychology, 137, 569–595.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J. (2003b). On the possible benefits of boredom: A neglected area in personality research. Psychology and Education: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 40, 28–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J., & Kass, S. J. (1990). A factor analytic study of the boredom proneness scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 115–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J., Verner, K. M., & Gilbride, T. V. (1991). Boredom proneness: Its relationship to positive and negative affect. Psychological Reports, 69, 1139–1146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J., & Watt, J. D. (2016). Self-report measures of boredom: An updated review of the literature. Journal of Psychology, 150, 196–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel-Walcutt, J. J., Fiorella, L., Carper, T., & Schatz, S. (2012). The definition, assessment, and mitigation of state boredom within educational settings: A comprehensive review. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 89–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watt, J. D., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1992). Relationship between boredom proneness and impulsivity. Psychological Reports, 70, 688–690.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., Visscher, K. M., & Woldorff, M. G. (2006). The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 971–978.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to James Danckert .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Danckert, J., Mugon, J., Struk, A., Eastwood, J. (2018). Boredom: What Is It Good For?. In: Lench, H. (eds) The Function of Emotions. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-77619-4_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics