Advertisement

Explanation Scaffolds Causal Learning and Problem Solving in Childhood

  • Justin T. A. Busch
  • Aiyana K. Willard
  • Cristine H. Legare
Chapter

Abstract

Explanation provides a window into what children know and scaffolds causal learning. Here we review research on the contributions of explanation to causal knowledge acquisition and problem solving. We discuss evidence that generating explanations enhances children’s understanding of causal mechanisms and increases their persistence and skill in applying new knowledge to novel contexts. In this way, explanation operates as a tool for learning and is particularly effective in the context of explaining inconsistent or ambiguous information. Explanation also enhances problem solving by allowing children to articulate their knowledge, a process which makes gaps in their current knowledge salient. The process of generating and requesting explanations facilitates the transmission of information and often occurs during interactions with others. We discuss the social context of explanation and the implications for belief revision and for building new knowledge. Explanation works in tandem with discovery-oriented behaviors like question asking and exploration to drive causal learning and improve problem solving.

References

  1. Amsterlaw, J., & Wellman, H. M. (2006). Theories of mind in transition: A microgenetic study of the development of false belief understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(2), 139–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baillargeon, R. (2002). The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy: A summary in eight lessons. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 47–83). Oxford, England: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bonawitz, E. B., Fischer, A., & Schulz, L. E. (2012). Teaching the Bayesian child: Three-and-a-half-year-olds’ reasoning about ambiguous evidence. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13(2), 266–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bonawitz, E. B., & Lombrozo, T. (2012). Occam’s Rattle: Children’s use of simplicity and probability to constrain inference. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1156–1164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bonawitz, E. B., Shafto, P., Gweon, H., Goodman, N. D., Spelke, E., & Schulz, L. (2011). The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery. Cognition, 120, 322–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bonawitz, E. B., van Schijndel, T., Friel, D., & Schulz, L. (2012). Balancing theories and evidence in children’s exploration, explanations, and learning. Cognitive Psychology, 64(4), 215–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brewer, W. F., Chinn, C. A., & Samarapungavan, A. (1998). Explanation in scientists and children. Minds and Machines, 8(1), 119–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Busch, J. T. A., Eck, E. A., Mercier, H., & Legare, C. H. (2016). Argumentation and the development of deductive reasoning abilities in childhood. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Philosophy and Psychology, Austin, TX.Google Scholar
  9. Busch, J.T.A., & Legare, C.H. (2016). The development of reasoning about evidence. Paper presented at the International Conference on Thinking, Providence, RI.Google Scholar
  10. Callanan, M. A., & Oakes, L. A. (1992). Preschoolers’ questions and parents’ explanations: Causal thinking in everyday activities. Cognitive Development, 7, 213–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Chouinard, N. M. (2007). Children’s questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monographs for the Society of Child Development, 72(1), 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cimpian, A., & Petro, G. (2014). Building theory-based concepts: Four-year-olds preferentially seek explanations for features of kinds. Cognition, 131(2), 300–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cook, C., Goodman, N. D., & Schulz, L. E. (2011). Where science starts: Spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’ exploratory play. Cognition, 120(3), 341–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Corriveau, K. H., & Kurkul, K. E. (2014). Why does rain fall? Children prefer to learn from an informant who uses noncircular explanations. Child Development, 85(5), 1827–1835.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Courage, M. L. (1989). Children’s inquiry strategies in referential communication and in the game of Twenty Questions. Child Development, 60, 877–886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Crowley, K., Callanan, M. A., Tenenbaum, H. R., & Allen, E. (2001). Parents explain more often to boys than to girls during shared scientific thinking. Psychological Science, 12, 258–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fawcett, L. M., & Garton, A. F. (2005). The effect of peer collaboration on children’s problem-solving ability. Educational Psychology, 75(2), 157–169.Google Scholar
  19. Foster, M. I., & Keane, M. T. (2015). Why some surprises are more surprising than others: Surprise as a metacognitive sense of explanatory difficulty. Cognitive Psychology, 81, 74–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Frazier, B. N., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2009). Preschoolers’ search for explanatory information within adult-child conversation. Child Development, 80, 1592–1611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Frazier, B. N., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2016). Young children prefer and remember satisfying explanations. Journal of Cognition and Development, 17(5), 718–736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gopnik, A. (2000). Explanation as orgasm and the drive for causal understanding: The evolution, function and phenomenology of the theory-formation system. In F. Keil & R. Wilson (Eds.), Cognition and explanation (pp. 299–323). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  23. Gopnik, A., & Schulz, L. E. (2007). Causal learning: Psychology, philosophy, and computation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gweon, H., Pelton, H., Konopka, J. A., & Schulz, L. E. (2014). Sins of omission: Children selectively explore when teachers are under-informed. Cognition, 132(3), 335–341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hempel, C. G. (1965). Aspects of scientific explanation and other essays in the philosophy of science. New York, NY: The Free Press.Google Scholar
  26. Hickling, A. K., & Wellman, H. M. (2001). The emergence of children’s causal explanations and theories: Evidence from everyday conversation. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 668–663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hilton, D. J. (1988). Logic and causal attribution. In D. J. Hilton (Ed.), Contemporary science and natural explanation: Commonsense conceptions of causality (pp. 33–65). Brighton, England: Harvester Press.Google Scholar
  28. Hilton, D. J. (1995). The social context of reasoning: Conversational inference and rational judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 248–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hoyos, C., & Gentner, D. (2017). Generating explanations via analogical comparison. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 24(5), 1364–1374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Keil, F. C. (2006). Explanation and understanding. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 227–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Keil, F. C., & Wilson, R. A. (2000). Explanation and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  32. Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Science, 38, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lancy, D. F. (2008). The anthropology of childhood: Cherubs, chattel, changelings. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Legare, C. H. (2012). Exploring explanation: Explaining inconsistent information guides hypothesis-testing behavior in young children. Child Development, 83, 173–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Legare, C. H. (2014). The contributions of explanation and exploration to children’s scientific reasoning. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 101–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Legare, C. H. (2017). Cumulative cultural learning: Diversity and development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 7877–7883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Legare, C. H., & Gelman, S. A. (2014). Examining explanatory biases in young children’s biological reasoning. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(2), 287–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Legare, C. H., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2010). Inconsistency with prior knowledge triggers children’s causal explanatory reasoning. Child Development, 81(3), 929–944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Legare, C. H., & Harris, P. L. (2016). The ontogeny of cultural learning. Child Development, 87(3), 633–642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Legare, C. H., & Lombrozo, T. (2014). Selective effects of explanation on learning during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 198–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Legare, C. H., Mills, C. M., Souza, A. L., Plummer, L. E., & Yasskin, R. (2013). The use of questions as problem-solving strategies during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 63–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Legare, C. H., Schult, C., Impola, M., & Souza, A. L. (2016). Young children revise explanations in response to new evidence. Cognitive Development, 39, 45–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Legare, C. H., Sobel, D. M., & Callanan, M. (2017). Causal learning is collaborative: Examining explanation and exploration in social contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(5), 1548–1554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Legare, C. H., Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). Evidence for an explanation advantage in naïve biological reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 177–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Lipton, P. (2004). Inference to the best explanation. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
  46. Lombrozo, T. (2006). The structure and function of explanations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(10), 464–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lombrozo, T. (2007). Simplicity and probability in causal explanation. Cognitive Psychology, 55, 232–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. McEldoon, K. L., Durkin, K. L., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). Is self-explanation worth the time? A comparison to additional practice. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(4), 615–632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mercier, H., Deguchi, M., Van der Henst, J.-B., & Yama, H. (2016). The benefits of argumentation are cross-culturally robust: The case of Japan. Thinking and Reasoning, 22(1), 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mercier, H., & Sperber, D. (2011). Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 57–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mercier, H., & Sperber, D. (2017). The enigma of reason. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Mills, C. M., Legare, C. H., Bills, M., & Mejias, C. (2010). Preschoolers use questions as a tool to acquire knowledge from different sources. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11(4), 533–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Mills, C. M., Legare, C. H., Grant, M. G., & Landrum, A. R. (2011). Determining who to question, what to ask, and how much information to ask for: The development of inquiry in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 539–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Piaget, J. (1929). The child’s conception of the world. London, England: Routledge & K. Paul.Google Scholar
  55. Rittle-Johnson, B., Saylor, M., & Swygert, K. E. (2008). Learning from explaining: Does it matter if mom is listening? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100, 215–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2007). Does comparing solutions methods facilitate conceptual and procedural knowledge? An experimental study on learning to solve equations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 561–574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  58. Ruggeri, A., & Lombrozo, T. (2015). Children adapt their questions to achieve efficient search. Cognition, 143, 203–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Schulz, L. (2012). The origins of inquiry: Inductive inference and exploration in early childhood. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16(7), 382–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Stahl, A. E., & Feigenson, L. (2015). Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration. Science, 348(6230), 91–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Teasley, S. D. (1997). Talking about reasoning: How important is the peer in peer collaboration? In L. B. Resnick, R. Saljo, C. Pontecorvo, & B. Burge (Eds.), Discourse, tools, and reasoning: Essays on situated cognition. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
  62. Trouche, E., Sander, E., & Mercier, H. (2014). Arguments, more than confidence, explain the good performance of reasoning in groups. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(5), 1958–1971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Walker, C. M., Lombrozo, T., Legare, C. H., & Gopnik, A. (2014). Explaining prompts children to privilege inductively rich properties. Cognition, 133, 343–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Walker, C. M., Lombrozo, T., Williams, J. J., Rafferty, A. N., & Gopnik, A. (2017). Explaining constrains causal learning in childhood. Child Development, 88(1), 229–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Walker, C. M., Williams, J. J., Lombrozo, T., & Gopnik, A. (2012). Explaining influences children’s reliance on evidence and prior knowledge in causal induction. In N. Miyake, D. Peebles, & R. P. Cooper (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1114–1119). Austin, TX.Google Scholar
  67. Wellman, H. M. (2011). Reinvigorating explanations for the study of early cognitive development. Child Development Perspectives, 5(1), 33–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Wellman, H. M., Hickling, A. K., & Schult, C. A. (1997). Young children’s psychological, physical, and biological explanations. New Directions for Child Development, 75, 7–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Wilkenfeld, D. A., & Lombrozo, T. (2015). Inference to the best explanation (IBE) versus explaining for the best inference (EBI). Science and Education, 24(9-10), 1059–1077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Willard, A. K., Busch, J. T. A., Sobel, D. M., Callanan, M. A., & Legare C. H. (2016). The impact of parent and children’s explanation and exploration on children’s causal learning. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Philosophy and Psychology, Austin, TX.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Justin T. A. Busch
    • 1
  • Aiyana K. Willard
    • 2
    • 3
  • Cristine H. Legare
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyThe University of Texas at AustinAustinUSA
  2. 2.Brunel University LondonUxbridgeUK
  3. 3.School of Anthropology and Museum Ethnography, University of OxfordOxfordUK

Personalised recommendations