Abstract
An unmade bed. A cigarette glued to the wall. A replica of a soup can box. Drippings on a canvas. Can an evolutionary approach help us understand the production and appreciation of, sometimes perplexing, modern and contemporary art? This chapter attempts at this by investigating two hypotheses about the evolution of human aesthetics in the domain of art. The first hypothesis, commonly called evolutionary aesthetics, asserts that aesthetic preferences, such as those for particular faces, body shapes and animals, have evolved in our ancestors because they motivated adaptive behavior. Artworks (e.g., those depicting facial beauty) may exploit these ancestral aesthetic preferences. In contrast, the second hypothesis states that aesthetic preferences continuously coevolve with artworks, and that they are subject to learning from, especially prestigious, other individuals. We called this mechanism prestige-driven coevolutionary aesthetics. Here I report artistic fieldwork and psychological experiments we conducted. We found that while exploitation of ancestral aesthetic preferences prevails among non-experts, prestige-driven coevolutionary aesthetics dominate expert appreciation. I speculate that the latter mechanism can explain modern and contemporary art’s deviations from evolutionary aesthetics as well as the existence and persistence of its elusiveness. I also discuss the potential relevance of our findings to major fields studying aesthetics, that is, empirical aesthetics, and sociological and historical approaches to art.
Access this chapter
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
- 1.
Modern art includes artistic work produced during the period extending roughly from the 1860s to the 1970s and denotes the style and philosophy of the art produced during that era, while more recent artistic production is often called contemporary art (or postmodern art).
- 2.
To be clear, whether something is aesthetic or not, depends on the concept of the aesthetic, naturally. Does the concept also comprise the possibility of conceptual beauty in addition to the more traditional perceptual beauty? For instance, someone might find an “anti-aesthetic” statement, such as Duchamp’s influential artwork “Fountain” (an ordinary urinal) intellectually quite beautiful, in a similar way as someone might find a mathematical proof beautiful.
- 3.
This is not to say that other aspects might have played a role in the popularity of the bear as well, such as its monumentality.
- 4.
Next to the dual inheritance or “Californian” school, there is a “Paris” school of cultural evolutionists (i.e., cultural attraction theory) (Sterelny 2016). While the focus of the Paris school also lies on explaining the evolution of human behavior and culture, their ideas are closer to standard evolutionary psychology than the Californian school. Here I focus on the Californian school.
- 5.
- 6.
Also sometimes referred to as the “global art world,” the international artworld is an intricate international network of artists, dealers, auction houses, collectors, and institutions, predominantly engaged in modern and contemporary art (e.g., Hart 1995).
- 7.
We showed this using mediation analyses. A mediation is a hypothesized causal chain in which one variable affects a second variable that, in turn, affects a third variable. The intervening variable, M, is the mediator. It “mediates” the relationship. In psychology, mediation is commonly used to investigate hypothesized “underlying” mechanisms.
- 8.
The fact that appreciation varies substantially between individuals does not preclude underlying human universals. Evolutionary researchers stress that human universal psychological characteristics are most often plastic, which means that they can change adaptively in response to particular environments.
References
Altman, M. N., Khislavsky, A. L., Coverdale, M. E., & Gilger, J. W. (2016). Adaptive attention: How preference for animacy impacts change detection. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(4), 303–314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.01.006.
Barrett, H. C. (2015). Adaptations to predators and prey. In The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 1–18). Hoboken, NJ, USA: Wiley. http://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125563.evpsych109.
Bloom, P. (2010). How pleasure works: The new science of why we like what we like. Random House.
Bourdieu, P. (1979). Le sens commun: La distinction critique sociale du jugement. Paris: Les Editions de Minuit.
Boyd, B. (2009). On the origin of stories: Evolution, cognition, and fiction. Harvard University Press.
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press.
Bullot, N. J., & Reber, R. (2013). The artful mind meets art history: Toward a psycho-historical framework for the science of art appreciation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(2), 123–137. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000489.
Cupchik, G. C., & László, J. (1992). Emerging visions of the aesthetic process: In psychology, semiology, and philosophy. Cambridge University Press.
Danto, A. (1964). The artworld. The Journal of Philosophy, 61(19), 571–584.
Davies, S. (2012). The artful species. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dissanayake, E. (1988). What is art for? Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Dissanayake, E. (1995). Homo aestheticus: Where art comes from and why. Seattle: University of Chicago Press.
Dissanayake, E. (1998). Komar and melamid discover pleistocene Taste. Philosophy and Literature, 22(2), 486–496. https://doi.org/10.1353/phl.1998.0039.
Dutton, D. (2003). Aesthetics and evolutionary psychology. In J. Levinson (Ed.), The Oxford handbook for aesthetics. Oxford University Press.
Dutton, D. (2009). The art instinct: Beauty, pleasure, and human evolution. Bloomsbury Press.
Falk, J. H., & Balling, J. D. (2010). Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference. Environment and Behavior, 42(4), 479–493. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916509341244.
Fitch, W. T. (2006). The biology and evolution of music: A comparative perspective. Cognition, 100(1), 173–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.009.
Garcia, C. M., & Lemus, Y. S. (2012). Foraging costs drive female resistance to a sensory trap. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1736), 2262–2268. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.2611.
Garcia, & Ramirez, E. (2005). Evidence that sensory traps can evolve into honest signals. Nature, 434, 501–505. http://doi.org/10.1038/nature03351.
Gombrich, E. (1951). The story of art. London: Phaidon.
Grosenick, U., & Riemschneider, B. (Eds.). (2005). Art now: 81 artists at the rise of the new millennium. Köln: Taschen.
Hart, L. M. (1995). Three walls: Regional aesthetics and the international art world. In The traffic in culture: Refiguring art and anthropology (pp. 127–150).
Headland, T., Pike, K., & Harris, M. (Eds.). (1990). Emics and etics: The insider/outsider debate. Newbury Park: Sage.
Henrich, J., & Broesch, J. (2011). On the nature of cultural transmission networks: Evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1139–1148. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0323.
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(3), 165–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00071-4.
Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.10110.
Hodgson, D., & Watson, B. (2015). The visual brain and the early depiction of animals in Europe and Southeast Asia. World Archaeology, 47(2014), 776–791. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00438243.2015.1074871.
Joye, Y., & De Block, A. (2011). “Nature and I are two”: A critical examination of the biophilia hypothesis. Environmental Values, 20(2), 189–215. https://doi.org/10.3197/096327111X12997574391724.
Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A., & Augustin, D. (2004). A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 489–508. https://doi.org/10.1348/0007126042369811.
Leder, H., Gerger, G., Dressler, S. G., & Schabmann, A. (2012). How art is appreciated. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(1), 2–10. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026396.
Leder, H., & Nadal, M. (2014). Ten years of a model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments: The aesthetic episode—Developments and challenges in empirical aesthetics. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 443–464. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12084.
Lewens, T. (2015). Cultural evolution: Conceptual challenges. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary based research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1571), 1638–1659. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0404.
New, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(42), 16598–16603. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0703913104.
Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108(3), 483–522. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.108.3.483.
Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved responses to landscapes. In Adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 555–579). Oxford University Press. http://doi.org/citeulike-article-id:7333260.
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking.
Prum, R. O. (2013). Coevolutionary aesthetics in human and biotic artworlds. Biology and Philosophy, 28(5), 811–832. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-013-9389-8.
Renoult, J. P. (2016). The evolution of aesthetics: A review of models. Aesthetics and Neuroscience, 271–299. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-46233-2_17.
Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2010). Gene-culture coevolution in the age of genomics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(Suppl), 8985–8992. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914631107.
Schacht, A., Werheid, K., & Sommer, W. (2008). The appraisal of facial beauty is rapid but not mandatory. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(2), 132–142. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.8.2.132.
Sperber, D., & Hirschfeld, L. A. (2004). The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 40–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.002.
Sterelny, K. (2016). Cultural evolution in California and Paris. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.shpsc.2016.12.005.
Thompson, D. (2008). The $12 million stuffed shark: The curious economics of contemporary art. Palgrave Macmillan.
Thornhill, R. (2003). Darwinian aesthetics informs traditional aesthetics. In E. Voland & K. Grammer (Eds.), Evolutionary aesthetics (pp. 9–35). Berlin: Springer. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-07142-7.
Van de Cruys, S., & Wagemans, J. (2011). Putting reward in art: A tentative prediction error account of visual art. I-Perception, 2(9), 1035–1062. https://doi.org/10.1068/i0466aap.
Verpooten, J., & Dewitte, S. (2017). The conundrum of modern art. Human Nature, 28(1), 16–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9274-7.
Verpooten, J., & Nelissen, M. (2010). Sensory exploitation and cultural transmission: The late emergence of iconic representations in human evolution. Theory in Biosciences, 129(2–3), 211–221. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12064-010-0095-7.
Voland, E., & Grammer, K. (2003). Evolutionary aesthetics. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Windhager, S., Atzwanger, K., Bookstein, F. L., & Schaefer, K. (2011). Fish in a mall aquarium-An ethological investigation of biophilia. Landscape and Urban Planning, 99(1), 23–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2010.08.008.
Wolfe, T. (1975). The painted word. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Yang, J., Wang, A., Yan, M., Zhu, Z., Chen, C., & Wang, Y. (2012). Distinct processing for pictures of animals and objects: Evidence from eye movements. Emotion, 12(3), 540–551. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026848.
Acknowledgements
I thank Siegfried Dewitte, Julien Renoult, and Jeanne Bovet for their comments. I also thank Julien Renoult for inviting me to contribute to this volume.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature
About this chapter
Cite this chapter
Verpooten, J. (2018). Expertise Affects Aesthetic Evolution in the Domain of Art. In: Kapoula, Z., Volle, E., Renoult, J., Andreatta, M. (eds) Exploring Transdisciplinarity in Art and Sciences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-76054-4_16
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-76054-4_16
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-319-76053-7
Online ISBN: 978-3-319-76054-4
eBook Packages: Religion and PhilosophyPhilosophy and Religion (R0)