Skip to main content

Gender and Citizen Responses to Corruption among Politicians: The U.S. and Brazil

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Gender and Corruption

Abstract

Schwindt-Bayer, Esarey, and Schumacher evaluate whether voters perceive of comparable male and female candidates differently in terms of how likely they are to be involved in a corruption scandal and punish them differently when they are involved in corruption. We conducted survey experiments in two countries, the United States (with high electoral accountability) and Brazil (with moderate to low electoral accountability), to determine if differential treatment is the causal mechanism linking women’s representation and corruption. We find only weak and statistically uncertain evidence that citizens perceive women as less corruptible than men in both countries, and we find no evidence that they punish women more harshly than men for corruption scandals.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Both experiments received human subject approval from the Rice University Institutional Research Board (IRB). U.S. experiment: study number IRB-FY2017-332; Brazilian experiment: study number IRB-FY2016-607.

  2. 2.

    The U.S. scored 22 on the 2015 Freedom House Freedom of the Press ranking indicating that its press is “free.” It scored a 74 out of 100 (100 = clean) on the Transparency International Corruption Perceptions Index in 2014, ranking it the 16th cleanest government out of 167 countries. On Johnson and Wallack’s (2005) personalism index, it scored a 10 out of 13, with 13 being the most personalistic.

  3. 3.

    Respondents in this survey could choose to leave this or any other question blank.

  4. 4.

    Drawing from the language used in the National Election Study, the question asked for a respondent’s “gender,” not “sex.” Thus, we use “gender” to discuss this question and the findings in this section. The Brazilian experiment, by contrast, asked for a respondent’s “sex.”

  5. 5.

    Respondents could select from one of the following categories: less than high school degree; high school graduate (high school diploma or equivalent including GED); some college but no degree; associate degree in college (two-year); bachelor’s degree in college (four-year); master’s degree; doctoral degree; professional degree (JD, MD).

  6. 6.

    Respondents could select among the following categories, including the possibility of selecting multiple options: White/non-Hispanic, Black or African-American, American Indian or Alaska Native, Asian , Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander, Hispanic, or Other. The number of respondents who reported being Pacific Islander, Native American, or Other was so small that we classified all such responses as being in a combined “Other” category to use in our analyses. Because these are not mutually exclusive categories, Appendix Table 4.1 reports coefficients for all categories.

  7. 7.

    Respondents reported their state of residence. We then classified states as being in one of four regions: North, Midwest, Southeast, and West.

  8. 8.

    Respondents could select from one of the following four categories: very interested, somewhat interested, not very interest, and not interested at all.

  9. 9.

    Specifically, we employ difference of proportions tests using prop.test in R (R Core Team 2017). The chi-square values on which the difference of proportions tests is based use the Yates’ continuity correction, which is the default in R.

  10. 10.

    The Brazilian survey experiment was conducted in Portuguese. The translated prompt is the following: “Imagine you live in a neighborhood like yours, but in a different state. In that state, a [man/woman] from a moderate party (neither extreme right or extreme left) was just elected governor. In the past, the state [has never had a female governor/has had a female governor]. The new governor promises to create jobs, improve access to healthcare and education and fight crime and corruption. [His/Her] approval ratings are fairly high, and [he/she] has strong support from many citizens in the state.” The prompt described the governor as “moderate” to downplay the significance of party ideology. In Brazil, the main cleavage among parties is not left-right, but whether the party supports the executive party in power, so the experiment aimed to minimize party ideology in the prompts (Samuels and Zucco 2014).

  11. 11.

    Social class is rated on a six-point scale, with 1 = upper class and 6 = lower class. In our sample, the lowest category had no respondents in it.

  12. 12.

    Respondents could select from the following categories: north, northeast, southeast, south, and central west.

  13. 13.

    The race and political interest questions were: (1) Do you consider yourself white, black, brown, indigenous or yellow? (with respondents selecting only one of these categorical options or “other”), and (2) How interested are you in politics? (with answers on a four-point ordinal scale from “very interested” to “not interested at all”). Unlike the U.S. experiment, respondents in the Brazilian experiment could choose only one racial category; thus one category (white) is excluded in the analyses of Appendix Tables 4.2 and 4.3.

  14. 14.

    The sex distribution across treatment groups was the following (male–female): Male governor, no history: 55.2–44.8 percent; Male governor, history: 44.1–55.9 percent; Female governor, no history: 49.8–50.2 percent; Female governor, history: 51.0–49.0 percent. The chi-square test for independence was statistically significant at conventional levels (p = 0.0193) as a result of the sex distributions in the two groups given the male governor treatments not being well balanced. We also found some evidence of imbalance in whether subjects answered both manipulation checks correctly: a chi-square test for independence of treatment and manipulation checks was statistically significant (p < 0.001), with the largest difference being an apparent excess of those answering both questions correctly in the “no history, female” treatment. Finally, we found evidence of imbalance in whether subjects were thinking of a specific state or politician (discussed in the next paragraph): a chi-square test for independence between treatment and this question was statistically significant (p = 0.007), with respondents in the female governor treatments being more likely to be thinking of a specific state or politician than men.

  15. 15.

    For details on Netquest’s Brazilian panel characteristics, see www.netquest.com/papers/panelbook_en.pdf, page 3.

  16. 16.

    Note that there is no statistically significant gender difference in whether respondents got the manipulation questions correct. Sixty-six percent of women got both questions correct as did 64 percent of men (chi-squared test for difference of proportions p = 0.285).

  17. 17.

    Specifically, in a linear regression predicting corruptibility , a multiplicative interaction term between (a) a dummy identifying subjects who were thinking of Dilma Rousseff and (b) the female governor treatment is statistically insignificant for all subjects, subjects in the condition with a history of a female governor, and subjects in the condition with no history of a female governor.

References

  • Alatas, V., Cameron, L., Chaudhuri, A., Erkal, N., & Gangadharan, L. (2009). Gender, culture, and corruption: Insights from an experimental analysis. Southern Economic Journal, 75(3), 663–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, D., & Andersen, K. (1993). Gender as a factor in the attribution of leadership traits. Political Research Quarterly, 46, 527–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, T. D., & Beaulieu, E. (2014). Gender stereotypes and corruption: How candidates affect perceptions of election fraud. Politics & Gender, 10(3), 365–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, T. D., Beaulieu, E., & Saxton, G. W. (2018). Restoring trust in the police: Why female officers reduce suspicions of corruption. Governance, 31(1), 143–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boas, T. C. (2014). Pastor Paulo vs. Doctor Carlos: Professional titles as voting heuristics in Brazil. Journal of Politics in Latin America, 6, 39–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boas, T. C. (2016). Pastors for pinochet: Authoritarian stereotypes and voting for evangelicals in Chile. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 3(2), 197–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, K. (2004). Voting for women: How the public evaluates women candidates. Boulder: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, K. (2010). The impact of gender stereotyped evaluations on support for women candidates. Political Behavior, 32, 69–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, K. (2014). When does gender matter?: Women candidates and gender stereotypes in American elections. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dollar, D., Fisman, R., & Gatti, R. (2001). Are women really the “fairer” sex? Corruption and women in government. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 46, 423–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Esarey, J., & Chirillo, G. (2013). ‘Fairer sex’ or purity myth? Corruption, gender, and institutional context. Politics & Gender, 9(04), 361–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Esarey, J., & Schwindt-Bayer, L. A. (2017). Women’s representation, accountability, and corruption in democracies. British Journal of Political Science, 1–32, published online January 26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R. L., & Smith, E. R. A. N. (1998). The role of candidate sex in voter decision-making. Political Psychology, 19, 405–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Funk, C. L. (1996). The impact of scandal on candidate evaluations: An experimental test of the role of candidate traits. Political Behavior, 18, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huddy, L., & Terkildsen, N. (1993). Gender stereotypes and the perception of male and female candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37(1), 119–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, J. W., & Wallack, J. S. (2005). Electoral systems and the personal vote. http://polisci2.ucsd.edu/jwjohnson/espv.htm

  • McDermott, M. L. (1998). Race and gender cues in low-information elections. Political Research Quarterly, 51(4), 895–919.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, R. (Ed.). (2010). Cracking the highest glass ceiling: A global comparison of women’s campaigns for executive office. Santa Barbara: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. http://www.R-project.org/

  • Samuels, D., & Zucco, C. (2014). The power of partisanship in Brazil: Evidence from survey experiments. American Journal of Political Science, 58, 212–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanbonmatsu, K. (2002). Gender stereotypes and vote choice. American Journal of Political Science, 46(1), 20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwindt-Bayer, L. A. (2016). Does the presence of women in politics reduce corruption in Latin America? Issue Brief, 7/29/16. Houston: James A. Baker III Institute for Public Policy at Rice University, Latin America Initiative. http://www.bakerinstitute.org/media/files/files/226840a1/BI-Brief-072916-LAI_Corruption.pdf

  • Schwindt-Bayer, L. A., & Reyes-Housholder, C. (2017). Citizen responses to female executives: Is it sex, novelty, or both? Politics, Groups and Identities, 5(3), 373–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwindt-Bayer, L. A., & Tavits, M. (2016). Clarity of responsibility, accountability, and corruption. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swamy, A., Knack, S., Lee, Y., & Azfar, O. (2001). Gender and corruption. Journal of Development Economics, 64, 25–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winters, M. S., & Weitz-Shapiro, R. (2013). Lacking information or condoning corruption: When do voters support corrupt politicians? Comparative Politics, 45(4), 418–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Żemojtel-Piotrowska, M. A., Marganski, A., Baran, T., & Piotrowski, J. (2016). Corruption and sexual scandal: The importance of politician gender. Anales de Psicología/Annals of Psychology, 33, 133–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendix

Appendix

Table 4.1 Multivariate logit models for all respondents in the United States
Table 4.2 Multivariate logit models for all respondents in Brazil
Table 4.3 Multivariate logit models for respondents in Brazil who answered both manipulation checks correctly

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Schwindt-Bayer, L.A., Esarey, J., Schumacher, E. (2018). Gender and Citizen Responses to Corruption among Politicians: The U.S. and Brazil. In: Stensöta, H., Wängnerud, L. (eds) Gender and Corruption. Political Corruption and Governance. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-70929-1_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics