Skip to main content

The Dynamic Autonomous Artwork

Part of the Palgrave Studies in Sound book series (PASTS)

Abstract

A strong motivation behind Gadamer building his theory of aesthetics was his desire to recover the autonomy of the artwork, as he felt that art was being considered more and more a purely subjective experience unless propped up by some form of transcendentalism. In this chapter, I try to extend Gadamer’s views on autonomy by questioning his views on limited agency, exploring how this might be improved by taking an enactivist standpoint. In an enactivist standpoint, our encounters hold within themselves an uneasy relationship between individual self-generation and intersubjective dynamics, which relates well to the difficult relations involved in aesthetics that allow the negotiated subjective interpretation of an autonomous artwork.

Keywords

  • Enactivism
  • Autonomous artwork
  • Autopoiesis
  • Sense-making

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-66899-4_3
  • Chapter length: 21 pages
  • Instant PDF download
  • Readable on all devices
  • Own it forever
  • Exclusive offer for individuals only
  • Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • ISBN: 978-3-319-66899-4
  • Instant PDF download
  • Readable on all devices
  • Own it forever
  • Exclusive offer for individuals only
  • Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
Hardcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)

References

  • Arthos, John. 2011. Where Is Muthos Hiding in Gadamer’s Hermeneutics? In Gadamer and Ricoeur, ed. Francis J. Mootz III and George H. Taylor, 119–141. Or, the Ontological Privilege of Emplotment. London and New York: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barad, Karen. 2003. Posthumanist Performativity. Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society 28 (3): 801–831. https://doi.org/10.1086/345321.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bruineberg, Jelle, Julian Kiverstein, and Erik Rietveld. 2016. The Anticipating Brain Is Not a Scientist: The Free-Energy Principle From an Ecological-Enactive Perspective. Synthese 36 (3): 1–28. Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-016-1239-1.

  • Cioffi, Maria Cristina, Michael J. Banissy, and James W. Moore. 2016. ‘Am I Moving?’ An Illusion of Agency and Ownership in Mirror-Touch Synaesthesia. Cognition 146 (January): 426–430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.10.020.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, Andy. 2013. Whatever Next? Predictive Brains, Situated Agents, and the Future of Cognitive Science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (3): 181–204. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477.

  • ———. 2016. Surfing Uncertainty. New York: Oxford University Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, Antonio R. 2010. Self Comes to Mind : Constructing the Conscious Brain. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, Hanne. 2015. How We Affect Each Other: Michel Henry’s “Pathos-with” and the Enactive Approach to Intersubjectivity. Journal of Consciousness Studies 22 (1–2): 112–132. Imprint Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, Hanne, Anssi Peräkylä, and Melisa Stevanovic. 2016. The Co-Creation of Meaningful Action: Bridging Enaction and Interactional Sociology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371 (1693): 20150378. The Royal Society. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0378.

  • DeNora, Tia. 2000. Music in Everyday Life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Di Paolo, Ezequiel A., Thomas Buhrmann, and Xabier Barandiaran. 2017. Sensorimotor Life. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Di Paolo, Ezequiel A., Marieke Rhohde, and Hanne De Jaegher. 2010. Horizons for the Enactive Mind: Values, Social Interaction, and Play. In Enaction, ed. John R. Stewart, Ezequiel A. Di Paolo, and Olivier Gapenne, 33–88. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, Katie M., and David K. Bilkey. 2007. Amusia Is Associated with Deficits in Spatial Processing. Nature Neuroscience 10 (7): 915–921. Nature Publishing Group. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1925.

  • Felski, Rita. 2011. Context Stinks! New Literary History 42 (4): 573–591. https://doi.org/10.1353/nlh.2011.0045.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Friston, Karl. 2009. The Free-Energy Principle: A Rough Guide to the Brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13 (7): 293–301.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Gadamer, Hans-Georg. 1986. In The Relevance of the Beautiful and Other Essays, ed. Robert Bernasconi. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2004. Truth and Method. Translated by Joel Weinsheimer and Donald G. Marshall. London and New York: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, Shaun. 2008. Inference or Interaction: Social Cognition Without Precursors. Philosophical Explorations 11 (3): 163–174. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.1080/13869790802239227.

  • Gallagher, Shaun, and Micah Allen. 2016. Active Inference, Enactivism and the Hermeneutics of Social Cognition. Synthese 116 (4): 953. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-016-1269-8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, Erving. 1964. The Neglected Situation. American Anthropologist. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1964.66.suppl_3.02a00090/full.

  • ———. 1971. Relations in Public. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagmann, P., O. Sporns, N. Madan, L. Cammoun, R. Pienaar, V.J. Wedeen, R. Meuli, J.P. Thiran, and P.E. Grant. 2010. White Matter Maturation Reshapes Structural Connectivity in the Late Developing Human Brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107 (44): 19067–19072. National Academic Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1009073107.

  • Held, R., and Alan Hein. 1963. Movement-Produced Stimulation in the Development of Visually Guided Behaviors. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 56 (5): 872–876.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herbert, Ruth. 2011. Everyday Music Listening: Absorption, Dissociation and Trancing. Surrey: Ashgate Publishing, Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hohwy, Jakob. 2013. The Predictive Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Innis, Robert E. 2001. Perception, Interpretation, and the Signs of Art. The Journal of Speculative Philosophy 15 (1): 20–32. https://doi.org/10.1353/jsp.2001.0002.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • James, William. 1878. Remarks on Spencer’s Definition of Mind as Correspondence. The Journal of Speculative Philosophy 12 (1): 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, Joel. 2011. Enacting Musical Content. In Situated Aesthetics: Art Beyond the Skin, ed. Riccardo Manzotti, 63–85. Exeter: Imprint Academic.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, Marc D. 2000. The Promise of Dynamic Systems Approaches for an Integrated Account of Human Development. Child Development 71 (1): 36–43. Blackwell Publishers Inc. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00116.

  • Lövdén, Martin, Elisabeth Wenger, Johan Mårtensson, Ulman Lindenberger, and Lars Bäckman. 2013. Structural Brain Plasticity in Adult Learning and Development. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (9): 2296–2310.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Markoš, Anton. 2002. Readers of the Book of Life. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClary, Susan. 2002. Feminine Endings. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. 1964. Signs. Translated by R.C. McCleary. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2012. Phenomenology of Perception. Translated by D.A. Landes. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morin, Marie-Eve. 2012. Jean-Luc Nancy. Cambridge: Polity.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morison, Sara J., Elinor W. Ames, and Kim Chisholm. 1995. The Development of Children Adopted From Romanian Orphanages. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 41 (4): 411–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., and C. Trevarthen. 1985. Emotional Regulation of Interactions Between 2-Month-Olds and Their Mothers. In Social Perception in Infants, ed. Tiffany M. Field and Nathan A. Fox, 177–197. Norwood: Ablex Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nancy, Jean-Luc. 2001. The Evidence of Film. Translated by Abbas Kiarostami. Brussels: Yves Gevaert.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2005. The Ground of the Image. New York: Fordham University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neal, David T., and Tanya L. Chartrand. 2011. Embodied Emotion Perception Amplifying and Dampening Facial Feedback Modulates Emotion Perception Accuracy. Social Psychological and Personality Science 2 (6): 673–678.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, Richard E. 1969. Hermeneutics. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruhleder, K., and B. Jordan. 2001. Co-Constructing Non-Mutual Realities: Delay-Generated Trouble in Distributed Interaction. Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW) 10: 113–138.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Särkämö, Teppo, Mari Tervaniemi, Seppo Soinila, Taina Autti, Heli M. Silvennoinen, Matti Laine, and Marja Hietanen. 2009. Cognitive Deficits Associated with Acquired Amusia After Stroke: A Neuropsychological Follow-Up Study. Neuropsychologia 47 (12): 2642–2651. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.015.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Sestoft, Dorte Maria, Henrik Steen Andersen, Tommy Lillebæk, and Gorm Gabrielsen. 1998. Impact of Solitary Confinement on Hospitalization Among Danish Prisoners in Custody. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 21 (1): 99–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2527(97)00025-3.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, John P., Sammy Perone, and Aaron T. Buss. 2011. Twenty Years and Going Strong: A Dynamic Systems Revolution in Motor and Cognitive Development. Child Development Perspectives 5 (4): 260–266. Blackwell Publishing Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00194.x.

  • Stewart, John R., Olivier Gapenne, and Ezequiel A. Di Paolo, eds. 2010. Enaction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, Shogo. 2015. Intercorporeality as a Theory of Social Cognition. Theory & Psychology 25 (4): 455–472. London, UK: SAGE Publications. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354315583035.

  • Thelen, Esther, and Linda B. Smith. 1996. A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2006. Dynamic Systems Theories. In Handbook of Child Psychology, ed. William Damon, Richard M. Lerner, K. Ann Renninger, and Irving E. Sigel, vol. 1, 258–312. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, Evan, and Francisco J. Varela. 2001. Radical Embodiment: Neural Dynamics and Consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5 (10): 418–425. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01750-2.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Verspoor, Marjolyn, Kees De Bot, and Wander Lowie. 2011. A Dynamic Approach to Second Language Development. John Benjamins Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Christensen, J. (2018). The Dynamic Autonomous Artwork. In: Sound and the Aesthetics of Play. Palgrave Studies in Sound. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-66899-4_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-66899-4_3

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Palgrave Macmillan, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-66898-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-66899-4

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)