Advertisement

“That’s Deep!”: The Role of Being Moved and Feelings of Profundity in the Appreciation of Serious Narratives

  • Florian Cova
  • Julien Deonna
  • David Sander
Chapter

Abstract

Why do we enjoy sad and serious narratives? According to recent research in communication theory and media psychology, it is because such narratives provide us with an occasion to reflect on deep and meaningful truths about human lives. However, this “eudaimonic” appreciation of sad narratives is not purely cognitive. Here, we connect this literature to recent research in affective sciences on feelings of “being moved.” Drawing on both philosophical analysis and psychological data, we argue that, because it is elicited by the contemplation of important positive values, the positive emotion of “being moved” plays a major role in the “eudaimonic” appreciation of sad narratives. Not only is it part of the pleasure they give us, but it leads us to reflect deeper about them.

References

  1. Algoe, S., & Haidt, J. (2009). Witnessing excellence in action: The ‘other-praising’ emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 105–127.Google Scholar
  2. Aquino, K., McFerran, B., & Laven, M. (2011). Moral identity and the experience of moral elevation in response to acts of uncommon goodness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 703–718.Google Scholar
  3. Bartsch, A., Kalch, A., & Oliver, M. B. (2014). Moved to Think: The role of emotional media experiences in stimulating reflective thoughts. Journal of Media Psychology, 26, 125–140.Google Scholar
  4. Cova, F., & Deonna, J.A. (2014). Being moved. Philosophical Studies, 169, no. 3, 447–466.Google Scholar
  5. Cova, F., Deonna, J.A. (2017). Is elevation a way of being moved? Unpublished manuscript, University of Geneva.Google Scholar
  6. Cox, K. (2010). Elevation predicts domain-specific volunteerism 3 months later. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 333–341.Google Scholar
  7. Deonna, J. (2011). Etre ému. In C. Tappolet, F. Teroni, & A. Konzelmann Ziv (Eds.), Les Ombres de l’Âme: Penser les émotions négatives. Genève: Markus Haller.Google Scholar
  8. Eerola, T., Vuoskoski, J. K., & Kautiainen, H. (2016). Being moved by unfamiliar sad music is associated with high empathy. Frontiers in Psychology, 7:1176.Google Scholar
  9. Ellithorpe, M. E., Ewoldsen, D. R., & Oliver, M. B. (2015). Elevation (sometimes) increases altruism: Choice and number of outcomes in elevating media effects. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 4, no. 3, 236–255.Google Scholar
  10. Freeman, D., Aquino, K., & McFerran, B. (2009). Overcoming beneficiary race as an impediment to charitable donations: Social dominance orientation, the experience of moral elevation, and donation behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 72–84.Google Scholar
  11. Haidt, J. (2000). The positive emotion of elevation. Prevention and Treatment, 3, no. 1, Article 3C.Google Scholar
  12. Haidt, J. (2003). Elevation and the positive psychology of morality. In C. L. M. Keyes & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 275–289). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  13. Hanich, J., Wagner, V., Shah, M., Jacobsen, T., & Menninghaus, W. (2014). Why we like to watch sad films. The pleasure of being moved in aesthetic experiences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, no. 2, 130–143.Google Scholar
  14. Konečni, V. J. (2005). The aesthetic trinity: Awe, being moved, thrills. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 5, 27–44.Google Scholar
  15. Konečni, V. J., Wanic, R. A., & Brown, A. (2007). Emotional and aesthetic antecedents and consequences of music-induced thrills. The American Journal of Psychology, 120, 619–643.Google Scholar
  16. Kuehnast, M., Wagner, V., Wassiliwizky, E., Jacobsen, T., & Menninghaus, W. (2014). Being moved: linguistic representation and conceptual structure. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1242.Google Scholar
  17. Menninghaus, W., Wagner, V., Hanich, J., Wassiliwizky, E., Kuehnast, M., & Jacobsen, T. (2015). Towards a psychological construct of being moved. PloS one, 10, no. 6, e0128451.Google Scholar
  18. Naar, H. (2013). Art and Emotion In J. Fieser & B. Dowden (eds.), Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy.Google Scholar
  19. Oliver, M. B. (1993). Exploring the paradox of the enjoyment of sad films. Human Communication Research, 19, no. 3, 315–342.Google Scholar
  20. Oliver,M. B. (2008). Tender affective states as predictors of entertainment preference. Journal of Communication, 58, 40–61.Google Scholar
  21. Oliver, M. B., & Bartsch, A. (2010). Appreciation as audience response: Exploring entertainment gratifications beyond hedonism. Human Communication Research, 36, no. 1, 53–81.Google Scholar
  22. Schramm, H., & Wirth, W. (2010). Exploring the paradox of sad-film enjoyment: The role of multiple appraisals and meta-appraisals. Poetics, 38, no. 3, 319–335.Google Scholar
  23. Oliver, M. B., & Raney, A. A. (2011). Entertainment as pleasurable and meaningful: Identifying hedonic and eudaimonic motivations for entertainment consumption. Journal of Communication, 61, no. 5, 984–1004.Google Scholar
  24. Oliver, M. B., Hartmann, T., & Woolley, J. (2012). Elevation in response to entertainment portrayals of moral virtue. Human Communication Research, 38, 360–378.Google Scholar
  25. Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2015). On the reception and detection of pseudo-profound bullshit. Judgment and Decision Making, 10, no. 6, 549–563.Google Scholar
  26. Pohling, R., & Diessner, R. (2016). Moral elevation and moral beauty: A review of the empirical literature. Review of General Psychology, 20, no. 4, 412–425.Google Scholar
  27. Saroglou, V., Buxant, C., & Tilquin, J. (2008). Positive emotions as leading to religion and spirituality. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3, 165–173.Google Scholar
  28. Schnall, S., & Roper, J. (2012). Elevation puts moral value into action. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 373–378.Google Scholar
  29. Schnall, S., Roper, J., & Fessler, D. M. (2010). Elevation leads to altruistic behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 315–320.Google Scholar
  30. Schubert, T. W., Zickfeld, J. H., Seibt, B., & Fiske, A. P. (in press). Moment-to-moment changes in feeling moved match changes in closeness, tears, goosebumps, and warmth: time series analyses. Cognition and Emotion.Google Scholar
  31. Taruffi, L., & Koelsch, S. (2014). The paradox of music-evoked sadness: an online survey. PLoS One, 9, no. 10, e110490.Google Scholar
  32. Thomson, A., & Siegel, J. (2013). A moral act, elevation, and prosocial behavior: Moderators of morality. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8, 50–64.Google Scholar
  33. Tokaji, A. (2003). Research for determinant factors and features of emotional responses of “kandoh”(the state of being emotionally moved). Japanese Psychological Research, 45, no. 4, 235–249.Google Scholar
  34. Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1986). Exploring the entertainment experience. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Perspectives on media effects (pp. 303–324). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  35. Zuckerman, M. (1996). Sensation seeking and the taste for vicarious horror. In J. B. Weaver & R. Tamborini (Eds.), Horror films: Current research on audience preferences and reactions (pp. 147–160).Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Florian Cova
    • 1
  • Julien Deonna
    • 1
  • David Sander
    • 1
  1. 1.Swiss Centre for Affective SciencesUniversity of GenevaGenevaSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations