Skip to main content

Moral Cognition Empowers Spiritual Experience in Chinese World of Warcraft

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Religious Cognition in China

Part of the book series: New Approaches to the Scientific Study of Religion ((NASR,volume 2))

  • 345 Accesses

Abstract

How does a videogame—the Chinese version of World of Warcraft (WoW)—become “spiritual” (jingshen 精神) in the eyes of its players? The question is relevant to the cognitive science of religion (CSR) because in “WoW spirituality” a secular, foreign artifact comes to signify a quasi-religious specialness through processes operating largely outside of—and at times in spite of—the power structures (religious, political, familial) that would typically authorize such signification. It is therefore possible that players’ discourse on the spiritual value of gameplay draws upon a deeply intuitive, cognitively natural mode of human reasoning about inner spiritual phenomena and hence might have significance for CSR theorizing about emergent religiosity (nova religio). In this chapter I explore these possibilities as follows. First, I examine the gameworld as a site for positive moral experience—an essential component of WoW spirituality—and locate causal power in the game’s rich affordances for the intuitive moral reasoning systems identified by Jonathan Haidt and Craig Joseph’s “moral foundations theory.” Second, I align this causal power with recurrent themes in how players use jingshen to characterize gameplay, arguing that high levels of moral affect, engendered by the moral foundation systems engaged through gameplay, potentiate spiritual signification. Third, I suggest this moral affect is attributed spiritual significance when players use intuitive essentialist reasoning to interpret that affect. I argue that if essentialist reasoning is indeed directing moral affect into spiritual signification, this would help to explain several patterns in players’ attributions and rescindments of spiritual significance. In closing, I identify two contributions CSR can make to the study of emergent religiosity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    All testimonials marked T1 were collected in the first year of fieldwork. With the help of a research assistant, I distributed flyers to young adults in Internet cafes that invited WoW players (identified by our perusing the monitors in the cafés) to write testimonials about why they play the game and the roles they felt it played in the wider contexts of their offline lives. Participants received a WoW game card (value = RMB¥30, or approximately US$4.60 at time of fieldwork). The flyers asked for basic demographic information, but few players elected to provide any (hence age and sex are not provided alongside the T1 testimonials).

  2. 2.

    By “rarely” I mean rarely straightforwardly, in the sense of a debate which reveals the transitional criteria. In my experience, such public discourse on the spiritual value of gameplay was mostly confined to online forums, where it was uncommon save for occasions when an issue impacted the entire WoW community (e.g., in Summer 2009 WoW was temporarily shut down), and conversations about a few popular WoW machinima films, such as Warcraft Wisdom (see below).

  3. 3.

    A notable exception here is the popular machinima film Warcraft Wisdom (FalanorTheElf, 2010), to which my queries were occasionally forwarded. The Chinese translation of the film articulates a logic in which positive in-game social interactions empower players to be better people.

  4. 4.

    State media occasionally referred to WoW as “spiritual opium” (jingshen yapian), thereby associating gameplay with addiction and encroachment by Western powers. This association effected considerable interpretive constraint on player discourse.

  5. 5.

    For an overview, see Golub and Lingley (2008).

  6. 6.

    Even taking into account the lack of clear boundaries within the Three Teachings, the absence of particular authorizing discourses is significant here, as jingshen cannot always be used freely—that is, without contestation—beyond the aegis of such discourse. In this sense jingshen is comparable to its English counterpart, “spiritual.” In America, one might agree that playing basketball is conducive to the development of virtues championed by the Abrahamic traditions; yet to describe basketball as a “spiritual experience”—to say that basketball is “spiritual food” and the basketball court is your “spiritual homeland”—would typically fetch ridicule or a demand for deeper justification.

  7. 7.

    Following Taves (2009, p. 26), who argues “the idea of ‘specialness’ is one broader, more generic net that captures most of what people have in mind when they refer to ‘sacred’ ‘magical’ ‘spiritual,’ ‘mystical’ or ‘religious’ and then some.”

  8. 8.

    A supernormal stimulus is (1) an exaggerated version of a stimulus to which there is an existing response tendency that (2) elicits a stronger response than the original, unexaggerated stimulus. Barrett (2010) offers a list of manmade examples: “candy sweeter than any fruit; stuffed animals with eyes wider than any baby, pornography, propaganda about menacing enemies” (p. 4).

  9. 9.

    Haidt and Joseph note that this last moral foundation—for detecting and avoiding contaminants—likely did not evolve for social cooperative purposes but, rather, was later exapted to them (2004).

  10. 10.

    In recent years, legal cases wherein bystanders who tried to help an accident victim and who were later successfully sued by the accident victim have made headlines. As a result, many Chinese are wary about helping in emergency circumstances and my Chinese colleagues occasionally passed around online videos wherein bystanders watched but refused to help injured motorists or pedestrians.

  11. 11.

    S3 responses did feature discourse on fairness, though less frequently than the T1 and T2 responses.

References

  • Ahn, W. (1998). The role of causal status in determining feature centrality. Cognition, 69, 135–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, D. (2010). Supernormal stimuli: How primal urges overran their evolutionary purpose. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim, É. (1995 [1912]). The elementary forms of religious life [Formes élémentaires de la vie religieuse.] (K. Fields, Trans.). New York: Free Press. (Original work published 1912).

    Google Scholar 

  • FalanorTheElf. (2010, May 5). Warcraft wisdom. Retrieved from http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=5gTAc1O0LLw/

  • Fleming, A. S., Corter, C., Stallings, J., & Steiner, M. (2002). Testosterone and prolactin are associated with emotional responses to infant cries in new fathers. Hormones and Behavior, 42(4), 399–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1970). The order of things: An archaeology of the human sciences. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A. (2003). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughhead, J. W., Valdez, J. N., Griffin, M. D., et al. (2009). Baby schema modulates the brain reward system in nulliparous women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 106(22), 9115–9119. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811620106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golub, A., & Lingley, K. (2008). Just like the Qing empire: Internet addiction, MMOGs, and moral crisis in contemporary China. Games and Culture, 3(1), 59–75. doi:10.1177/1555412007309526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, 133(4), 55–66. doi:10.1162/0011526042365555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2007). The moral mind: How five sets of innate moral intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules. In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate mind (Vol. 3, pp. 367–391). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hornbeck, R. G. (Forthcoming). Pure war: Moral cognition and spiritual experience in Chinese World of Warcraft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hornbeck, R. G. (2015). Explaining time spent in multiplayer online games: Moral cognition in Chinese World of Warcraft. Games and Culture, 11(5). doi:10.1177/1555412015570112.

  • Huizinga, J. (1994). Homo Ludens. Boston: Beacon Press. (Original work published in 1944).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, C. P. (2007). A retrospective: Reflections on the tenth anniversary of Nova Religio. Nova Religio, 10(4), 8–16. doi:10.1525/nr.2007.10.4.8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, J. D. (2004). The primate isolation call: A comparison with precocial birds and nonprimate mammals. In L. J. Rogers & G. Kaplan (Eds.), Comparative vertebrate cognition: Are primates superior to non-primates? (pp. 171–187). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pertaub, D. P., Slater, M., & Barker, C. (2002). An experiment on public speaking anxiety in response to three different types of virtual audience. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 11(1), 68–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richert, R. A., & Harris, P. L. (2008). Dualism revisited: Body vs mind vs. Soul. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8, 99–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richert, R. A., & Smith, E. (2012). The essence of soul concepts: How soul concepts influence ethical reasoning across religious affiliation. Religion, Brain, & Behavior, 2(2), 161–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salen, K., & Zimmerman, E. (2004). Rules of play: Game design fundamentals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strohminger, N., & Nichols, S. (2014). The essential moral self. Cognition, 131(1), 159–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tribe, L. H. (1972). Policy science: Analysis or ideology? Philosophy and Public Affairs, 2(1), 66–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yee, N. (2014). The proteus paradox: How online games and virtual worlds change us—And how they don’t. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zak, P. J. (2012). The moral molecule: The source of love and prosperity. New York: Dutton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, T. E., Prudom, S. L., Zahed, S. R., Parlow, A. F., & Wegner, F. (2009). Prolactin’s mediative role in parenting in parentally experienced marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Hormones and Behaviors, 56(4), 436–443. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.07.012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ryan G. Hornbeck .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hornbeck, R.G. (2017). Moral Cognition Empowers Spiritual Experience in Chinese World of Warcraft . In: Hornbeck, R., Barrett, J., Kang, M. (eds) Religious Cognition in China. New Approaches to the Scientific Study of Religion , vol 2. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-62954-4_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics