Advertisement

General Epistemology of Open Systems

Chapter
Part of the SpringerBriefs in Psychology book series (BRIEFSPSYCHOL)

Abstract

Three basic rules (derived from the axioms outlined in Chap.  1) are outlined:
  1. 1.

    All human psychological processes unfold in irreversible time (the rule of time).

     
  2. 2.

    All human psychological processes—while being based on the unity of the body and the psyche—are being generated in relation with the environment (the rule of the open systems).

     
  3. 3.

    All human psychological processes are generated with the guidance of teleogenetic constructions by the person in coordination with social expectations (the rule of goal orientations).

     

These three rules set stringent constraints upon methodology. While the rule of time lets psychological processes share common ground with nonequilibrium thermodynamics (Prigogine 1977), the rule of open systems guarantees unity or psychology with all biological systems. Finally, the rule of goal orientations outlines the specifically human aspect of the psychological processes that involve constant preadaptation by the person toward the not-yet-knowable future. It is here where imagination, affect, and reasoning converge. The chapter includes discussions of the meaning of the data.

References

  1. Bachelard, G. (2002). The formation of the scientific mind. Manchester: Clinamen Press [original in 1938].Google Scholar
  2. Baldwin, J. M. (1930). James Mark Baldwin. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 1, pp. 1–30). New York: Russell & Russell.Google Scholar
  3. Bertalanffy, L. (1940). Die Organismus als physikalisches System. Naturwissenschaften, 26, 521–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bertalanffy, L. (1950). The theory of open systems in physics and biology. Science, 111, 23–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bertalanffy, L. (1951). Theoretical models in biology and psychology. Journal of Personality, 20, 24–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General systems theory. New York: Beorge Braziller.Google Scholar
  7. Diesner, T. (2015). Die Allgemeine Systemtheorie bei Ludwig von Bertalanffy. Berlin: Logos.Google Scholar
  8. Drack, M., Apfalter, W., & Pouvreau, D. (2007). On the making of a system theory of life: Paul A Weiss and Ludwig von Bertalanffy’s conceptual connection. Quarterly Review of Biology, 82(4), 349–374.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Driesch, H. (1905). Der Vitalismus als Geschichte und als Lehre. Leipzig: J.A. Barth.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Glaveanu, V.-P. (Ed.). (2016). The Palgrave handbook of creativity and culture research. London: Palgrave.Google Scholar
  11. Kvale, S. (1976). Facts and dialectics. In J. F. Rychlak (Ed.), Dialectic: Humanistic rationale for behavior and development (pp. 87–100). Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
  12. Löwy, I. (1996). Between bench and bedside. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Maruyama, M. (1963). The second cybernetics: Deviation-amplifying mutual causal processes. American Scientist, 51, 164–179.Google Scholar
  14. Porter, T. (2003). The social sciences. In D. Cahan (Ed.), From natural Philosophy to the sciences (pp. 254–290). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  15. Pouvreau, D. (2009). The dialectical tragedy of the concept of wholeness: Ludwig von Bertalanffy’s biography revisited . Vol. 1. Exploring unity through diversity. Litchfield Park, AZ: ICSE Publications.Google Scholar
  16. Pouvreau, D., & Drack, M. (2007). On the history of Ludwig von Bertalanffy’s “general Systemology”, and on its relationship to cybernetics. Part I. Elements on the origins and genesis of Ludwig von Bertalanffy’s “general Systemology”. International Journal of General Systems, 36, 3281–3337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Prigogine, I. (1977). Time, structure and fluctuations. The Nobel lecture on December, 8. Stockholm: The Nobel Foundation.Google Scholar
  18. Toomela, A., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2010). Methodological thinking in psychology: 60 years gone astray? Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.Google Scholar
  19. Valsiner, J. (1984). Two alternative epistemological frameworks in psychology: The typological and variational modes of thinking. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 5(4), 449–470.Google Scholar
  20. Valsiner, J. (Ed.). (2005). Heinz Werner and developmental science. New York: Kluwer Scientific/Plenum Publishers.Google Scholar
  21. Valsiner, J. (2014). Invitation to cultural psychology. London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Valsiner, J. (2017). Making sense of self-completing wholes: Epistemological travels of Hans Driesch. In D. Carre, J. Valsiner, & S. Hampl (Eds.), Representing development: The social construction of models of change (pp. 28–48). Abington, Ox-UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
  23. Valsiner, J., & Brinkmann, S. (2016). Beyond the “variables”: Developing metalanguage in psychology. In S. H. Klempe & R. Smith (Eds.), Centrality of history for theory construction in psychology (pp. 75–90). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Communication and PsychologyAalborg UniversityAalborgDenmark

Personalised recommendations