Skip to main content

Lay Theories of the Mind/Brain Relationship and the Allure of Neuroscience

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Science of Lay Theories

Abstract

Psychology is in the midst of a neuroscience revolution, with attributes deemed uniquely human—language, free will, moral responsibility, a notion of self—no longer exempt from material exploration. It is anybody’s guess whether this neuroscience endeavor to explain the human mind will succeed. In the meantime, what does seem clear is that neuroscience is everywhere these days, and that people—both expert and ordinary folk—are intrigued by its role in explaining the mind. Why is neuroscience so alluring? To ask this question is to ask about current lay theories of the mind/brain relation. To approximate an answer, I start by describing the allure of neuroscience (brain) explanations for psychological (mind) phenomena. Is the allure due to neuroscience’s status as a ‘real’ science? Or is it due to its reductive appeal? Next, I review the effect of neuroscience on judgments of responsibility and free will. Do ordinary folk shy away from free will and moral condemnation when actions are couched in terms of brain function? The next stop on our tour of uniquely human capacities is the notion of the self. Do ordinary folk believe that their brains are who they truly are? I conclude by exploring possible policy implications of the allure of neuroscience. How might it affect our funding of science, our education policy, and our mental health system?

Allure: the quality of being powerfully and mysteriously attractive or fascinating.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    These contortions include rejecting determinism to save free will (libertarians); conceding that the world is deterministic and thus acknowledging that that free will does not exist (hard determinists); or accepting determinism but still claim that free will is possible (i.e., compatibilism).

References

  • Ahn, W. K., Proctor, C. C., & Flanagan, E. H. (2009). Mental health clinicians’ beliefs about the biological, psychological, and environmental bases of mental disorders. Cognitive Science, 33(2), 147–182. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01008.x

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Anglin, S. M. (2014). I think, therefore I am? Examining conceptions of the self, soul, and mind. Consciousness and Cognition, 29, 105–116. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.08.014

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aspinwall, L. G., Brown, T. R., & Tabery, J. (2012). The double-edged sword: does biomechanism increase or decrease judges’ sentencing of psychopaths? Science, 337(6096), 846–849. doi:10.1126/science.1219569

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beall, E., Meindl, P., Iyer, R., Monterosso, J., Graham, J., & Schwarz, B. (2013). Biology or bad character? Brain scan images increase determinism and decrease moral attributions for criminal acts. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, New Orleans, LA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bering, J. M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2004). The natural emergence of reasoning about the afterlife as a developmental regularity. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 217–233. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.217

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertossa, F., Besa, M., Ferrari, R., & Ferri, F. (2008). Point zero: A phenomenological inquiry into the seat of consciousness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 107(2), 323–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, Paul. (2004). Descartes’ baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, J. S. (2016). The practical and principled problems with educational neuroscience. Psychological Review, 123(5), 600–612. doi:10.1037/rev0000025

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broca, P. (1861). Remarks on the seat of the faculty of articulated language, following an observation of aphemia (loss of speech). Remarques sur le siége de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d’une observation d’aphémie (perte de la parole). Bulletin de la Société Anatomique, 6, 330–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B., Varma, S., & Laurillard, D. (2011). Dyscalculia: From brain to education. Science, 332(6033), 1049–1053. doi:10.1126/science.1201536

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The science and evolution of a human invention. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2011). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics (Rev. and updated ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demertzi, A., Liew, C., Ledoux, D., Bruno, M. A., Sharpe, M., Laureys, S., et al. (2009). Dualism persists in the science of mind. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1157, 1–9. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2008.04117.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Descartes, R. (1641) (1984/1641). Meditations on first philosophy. In (Cottingham, Trans.) R. J. Stoothoff & D. Murdoch (Eds.), The philosophical writings of rené descartes (Vol. 2, pp. 1–62). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dronkers, N. F., Plaisant, O., Iba-Zizen, M. T., & Cabanis, E. A. (2007). Paul Broca’s historic cases: High resolution MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong. Brain, 130(Pt 5), 1432–1441. doi:10.1093/brain/awm042

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksson, K. (2012). The nonsense math effect. Judgment and Decision Making, 7(6), 746–749.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque, D., & Black, S. E. (2007). Metacognitive judgment and denial of deficit: Evidence from frontotemporal dementia. Judgment and Decision Making, 2(6), 359–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque, D., Evans, J., Christian, C., & Hodges, S. D. (2015). Superfluous neuroscience information makes explanations of psychological phenomena more appealing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(5), 926–944. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00750

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque, D., Hodges, S. D., Baird, J. A., & Black, S. E. (2010). Empathy in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(3), 289–298. doi:10.1080/13803390903002191

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque, D., & Schwartz, B. (2016). Common sense beliefs about the central self, moral character, and the brain. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02007

  • Gabrieli, J. D., Ghosh, S. S., & Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. (2015). Prediction as a humanitarian and pragmatic contribution from human cognitive neuroscience. Neuron, 85(1), 11–26. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.047

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, G. P., Piazza, J., & Rozin, P. (2014). Moral character predominates in person perception and evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(1), 148–168. doi:10.1037/a0034726

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, H. M., Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Dimensions of mind perception. Science, 315(5812), 619. doi:10.1126/science.1134475

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greely, A. M., & Hout, M. (1999). Americans’ increasing belief in life after death: Religious competition and acculturation. American Sociological Review, 64, 813–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, E., & Cahill, B. S. (2012). Effects of neuroimaging evidence on mock juror decision making. Behaviral Sciences and the Law, 30(3), 280–296. doi:10.1002/bsl.1993

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J., & Cohen, J. (2004). For the law, neuroscience changes nothing and everything. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 359(1451), 1775–1785. doi:10.1098/rstb.2004.1546

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gurley, J. R., & Marcus, D. K. (2008). The effects of neuroimaging and brain injury on insanity defenses. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 26(1), 85–97. doi:10.1002/bsl.797

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, J. (1868). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. Massachusetts Medical Society, 2, 327–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, N. (2011). Genetic essentialism, neuroessentialism, and stigma: commentary on Dar-Nimrod and Heine (2011). Psychological Bulletin, 137(5), 819–824. doi:10.1037/a0022386

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, E. J., Weisberg, D. S., & Taylor, J. C. (2016). The seductive allure is a reductive allure: People prefer scientific explanations that contain logically irrelevant reductive information. Cognition, 155, 67–76. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2016.06.011

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C. N., & Wellman, H. M. (1982). Children’s developing conceptions of the mind and brain. Child Development, 53(1), 222–234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C., Lockhart, K. L., & Schlegel, E. (2010). A bump on a bump? Emerging intuitions concerning the relative difficulty of the sciences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(1), 1–15. doi:10.1037/a0018319

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, N. S., Ahn, W. K., Johnson, S. G., & Knobe, J. (2016). The influence of framing on clinicians’ judgments of the biological basis of behaviors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 22(1), 39–47. doi:10.1037/xap0000070

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knobe, J., & Prinz, J. (2008). Intuitions about consciousness: Experimental studies. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 7, 67–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lebowitz, M. S., & Ahn, W. K. (2014). Effects of biological explanations for mental disorders on clinicians’ empathy. Proceeding National Academy of Science U S A, 111(50), 17786–17790. doi:10.1073/pnas.1414058111

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lebowitz, M. S., Pyun, J. J., & Ahn, W. K. (2014). Biological explanations of generalized anxiety disorder: Effects on beliefs about prognosis and responsibility. Psychiatry Services, 65(4), 498–503. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.201300011

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lebowitz, M. S., Rosenthal, J. E., & Ahn, W. K. (2016). Effects of biological versus psychosocial explanations on stigmatization of children with ADHD. Journal of Attentional Disorders, 20(3), 240–250. doi:10.1177/1087054712469255

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lillard, A. (1998). Ethnopsychologies: Cultural variations in theories of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 123(1), 3–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markovitz, J. C. (2016). There is such a thing as too much neuroscience. New York Times, October 14. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2016/10/15/opinion/theres-such-a-thing-as-too-much-neuroscience.html

  • Markus, H., & Wurf, E. (1987). The dynamic self-concept: A social psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 299–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, D. P., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Seeing is believing: The effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition, 107(1), 343–352. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.017.

  • McCloskey, M., & Kohl, D. (1983). Naive physics: The curvilinear impetus principle and its role in interactions with moving objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9(1), 146–156.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, R. B., Newman, E. J., Vuorre, M., Cumming, G., & Garry, M. (2013). On the (non) persuasive power of a brain image. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(4), 720–725. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0391-6

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monroe, A. E., Brady, G., & Malle, B. F. (2016). This isn’t the free will worth looking for: General free will beliefs do not influence moral judgments, agent-specific choice ascriptions do. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 27, 100–108. doi:10.1177/1948550616667616

    Google Scholar 

  • Monroe, A. E., & Malle, B. F. (2010). From uncaused will to conscious choice: The need to study, not speculate about, people’s folk concept of free will. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 1, 211–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monterosso, J., Royzman, E. B., & Schwartz, B. (2005). Explaining away responsibility: Effects of scientific explanation on perceived culpability. Ethics and Behavior, 15(2), 139–158. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb1502_4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C., Lemmon, K., & Skene, K. (Eds.). (2001). The self in time: Developmental Perspectives. Mawah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nahmias, E. (2006). Folk fears about freedom and responsibility: Determinism vs. reductionism. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 6(1–2), 215–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nahmias, E. (2016). Free will as a psychological accomplishment. In D. Schmidtz & C. Pavel (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of freedom: OUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nahmias, E. (2017). Your brain as the source of free will worth wanting: Understanding free will in the age of neuroscience. In G. Caruso & O. Flanagan (Eds.), Neuroexistentialism: Meaning, morals, and purpose in the age of neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nahmias, E., Morris, S., Nadelhoffer, T., & Turner, J. (2005). Surveying freedom: Folk intuitions about free will and moral responsibility. Philosophical Psychology, 18, 561–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nahmias, E., Shepard, J., & Reuter, S. (2014). It’s OK if ‘my brain made me do it’: People’s intuitions about free will and neuroscientific prediction. Cognition, 133(2), 502–516. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.07.009

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1(1), 35–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, S. (2011). Experimental philosophy and the problem of free will. Science, 331(6023), 1401–1403. doi:10.1126/science.1192931

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, S., & Knobe, J. (2007). Moral responsibility and determinism: The cognitive science of folk intuitions. Noûs, 41(4), 663–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peetz, J., & Wilson, A. E. (2008). The temporally extended self: The relation of past and future selves to current identity, motivation, and goal pursuit. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 2090–2106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind: The third Paul M. Fitts lectures, delivered at the University of Michigan, September 1976. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. New York: Scientific American Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quoidbach, J., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2013). The end of history illusion. Science, 339(6115), 96–98. doi:10.1126/science.1229294

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Racine, E., Bar-Ilan, O., & Illes, J. (2005). fMRI in the public eye. Nature Review Neuroscience, 6(2), 159–164. doi:10.1038/nrn1609

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riekki, T., Lindeman, M., & Lipsanen, J. (2013). Conceptions about the mind-body problem and their relations to afterlife beliefs, paranormal beliefs, religiosity, and ontological confusions. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 112–120. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0138-5

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roskies, A. (2006). Neuroscientific challenges to free will and responsibility. Trends Cogn Sci, 10(9), 419–423. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.07.011

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. (2015). Why we work (First TED books hardcover edition September 2015. ed.).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweitzer, N. J., & Saks, M. J. (2011). Neuroimage evidence and the insanity defense. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 29(4), 592–607. doi:10.1002/bsl.995

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sedikides, C. (1995). Central and peripheral self-conceptions are differentially influenced by mood: tests of the differential sensitivity hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(4), 759–777.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shariff, A. F., Greene, J. D., Karremans, J. C., Luguri, J., Clark, C. J., Schooler, J. W., et al. (2014). Free will and punishment: A mechanistic view of human nature reduces retribution. Psychological Science, 25(8), 1563–1570. doi:10.1177/0956797614534693

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. A., & Rabb, N. (2016). Your understanding is my understanding: Evidence for a community of knowledge. Psychological Science, 27(11), 1451–1460. doi:10.1177/0956797616662271

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1989). Implicit philosophies of mind—The dualism scale and its relation to religiosity and belief in extrasensory perception. Journal of Psychology, 123(1), 5–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starmans, C., & Bloom, P. (2012). Windows to the soul: Children and adults see the eyes as the location of the self. Cognition, 123(2), 313–318. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strohminger, N., & Nichols, S. (2015). Neurodegeneration and Identity. Psychological Science, 26(9), 1469–1479. doi:10.1177/0956797615592381

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tal, A., & Wansink, B. (2014). Blinded with science: Trivial graphs and formulas increase ad persuasiveness and belief in product efficacy. Public Understanding of Science. doi:10.1177/0963662514549688

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Dell’Acqua, F., Ratiu, P., Leslie, A., Howells, H., Cabanis, E., … Catani, M. (2015). From Phineas Gage and Monsieur Leborgne to H.M.: Revisiting disconnection syndromes. Cerebral Cortex, 25(12), 4812–4827. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv173

  • Wakslak, C. J., Nussbaum, S., Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2008). Representations of the self in the near and distant future. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 757–773. doi:10.1037/a0012939

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Weisberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E., & Gray, J. R. (2008). The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3), 470–477. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20040

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2001). From chump to champ: People’s appraisals of their earlier and present selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(4), 572–584.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Diego Fernandez-Duque .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Fernandez-Duque, D. (2017). Lay Theories of the Mind/Brain Relationship and the Allure of Neuroscience. In: Zedelius, C., Müller, B., Schooler, J. (eds) The Science of Lay Theories. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-57306-9_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics