Advertisement

What’s Included in the Set of Alternatives?

  • Nicole Gotzner
Chapter
  • 176 Downloads
Part of the Palgrave Studies in Pragmatics, Language and Cognition book series (PSPLC)

Abstract

This chapter examines which specific elements are included in the set of alternatives. The leading research question is whether listeners determine alternatives based on general semantic priming mechanisms or whether they only consider contrastive alternatives, elements that can replace the expression in focus. Experiment 4 compares semantically related alternatives to general non-contrastive associates of a focused expression. [A detailed description of Experiment 4 is published in Gotzner and Spalek (Discourse Process, doi:10.1080/0163853X.2016.1148981, 2016).] [The data of Experiment 4 are published in Gotzner and Spalek (Discourse Process, doi:10.1080/0163853X.2016.1148981, 2016).] The results show that effects of focus particles are selective to alternatives which can replace the expression in focus. The second part of the chapter turns to the theoretical debate concerning the restriction of alternative sets. To address this debate, I present a further analysis of the unrelated items used in Experiment 3. [A version of Sect. 5.4 was published in the Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung (Gotzner, vol. 19, pp. 232–247, 2015). I designed the additional analysis presented in Sect. 5.4 as well as Experiment 4 and analyzed all results.] Overall, the results suggest that listeners consider a broader set of alternatives and that the notion of possible replacements is crucial in determining the relevant alternatives.

Keywords

Alternative sets Contrast Contextual restriction Semantic and taxonomic categories 

References

  1. Blok, P., & Eberle, K. (1999). What is the alternative? The computation of focus alternatives from lexical and sortal information. In P. Bosch & R. van der Sandt (Eds.), Focus: Linguistic, cognitive and computational perspectives (pp. 105–120). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  2. Braun, B., & Tagliapietra, L. (2010). The role of contrastive intonation contours in the retrieval of contextual alternatives. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 1024–1043.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Büring, D. (2008). What’s new (and what’s given) in the theory of focus? In Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (Vol. 34, pp. 403–423).Google Scholar
  4. Byram-Washburn, M. (2013). Narrowing the Focus: Experimental Studies on Exhaustivity and Contrast. Ph.D. thesis, University of Southern California.Google Scholar
  5. Cohen, A. (1999). How are alternatives computed? Journal of Semantics, 16, 43–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Fraundorf, S., Watson, D., & Benjamin, A. (2010). Recognition memory reveals just how contrastive contrastive accenting really is. Journal of Memory & Language, 63, 367–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Gotzner, N. (2015). What’s included in the set of alternatives? Psycholinguistic evidence for a permissive view. In E. Csipak & H. Zeijlstra (Eds.), Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung, Göttingen (Vol. 19, pp. 232–247).Google Scholar
  8. Gotzner, N., & Spalek, K. (2016). The role of contrastive and non-contrastive associates in the interpretation of focus particles. Discourse Processes, doi:10.1080/0163853X.2016.1148981.Google Scholar
  9. Husband, E. M., & Ferreira, F. (2016). The role of selection in the comprehension of focus alternatives. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31, 217–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Katzir, R. (2013). A note on contrast. Natural Language Semantics, 21, 333–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Kim, C. (2012). Generating Alternatives: Interpreting Focus in Discourse. Ph.D. thesis, University of Rochester.Google Scholar
  12. Krifka, M. (1998). Additive particles under stress. In Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory (SALT) (Vol. 8, pp. 111–128).Google Scholar
  13. Kristensen, L. B., Wang, L., Petersson, K. M., & Hagoort, P. (2013). The interface between language and attention: Prosodic focus marking recruits a general attention network in spoken language comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 1836–1848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Rooth, M. (1985). Association with Focus. Ph.D. thesis, Massachussets Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
  15. Rooth, M. (1992). A theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics, 1, 1–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Umbach, C. (2001). Restriktion der Alternativen. Linguistische Arbeitsberichte, 77, 165–198.Google Scholar
  17. Wagner, M. (2006). Givenness and locality. In Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory (SALT) (Vol. 16, pp. 295–312).Google Scholar
  18. Wagner, M. (2012). Focus and givenness: a unified approach. In Contrasts and Positions in Information Structure (pp. 102–147). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nicole Gotzner
    • 1
  1. 1.Humboldt UniversityBerlinGermany

Personalised recommendations