Advertisement

The Mechanisms of Activation and Competitive Inhibition

  • Nicole Gotzner
Chapter
  • 177 Downloads
Part of the Palgrave Studies in Pragmatics, Language and Cognition book series (PSPLC)

Abstract

This chapter investigates immediate representations of focus alternatives to explore the mechanisms underlying the establishment of alternative sets and the impact of focus particles on these mechanisms. In particular, Experiments 2 and 3 test the retrieval/activation of contextually mentioned alternatives and additional unmentioned alternatives (an earlier version of this chapter was published in Gotzner et al. Lang Cogn 8:59–95, 2016). [This chapter is a version of the article (Gotzner et al. Lang Cogn 8:59–95, 2016). Experiments 2 and 3 were designed by Katharina Spalek and myself. I analyzed all data and wrote the article.] The overall goal of the experiments is to assess the activation and competition among members of the alternative set. The first experiment uses a probe recognition task and shows that focus particles interfere with the recognition of mentioned alternatives and the rejection of unmentioned alternatives relative to a condition without a particle. A second lexical decision experiment demonstrated priming effects for mentioned and unmentioned alternatives (compared with an unrelated condition) while focus particles caused additional interference effects. Overall, our results indicate that focus particles trigger an active search for alternatives and lead to a competition between mentioned alternatives, unmentioned alternatives, and the focused element.

Keywords

Focus particles Alternative-set semantics Probe recognition task Lexical decision task Competitive inhibition 

References

  1. Baayen, H. (2008). Analyzing Linguistic Data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baddeley, A., Eysenck, M., & Anderson, M. (2009). Memory. East Sussex: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  3. Beaver, D., & Clark, B. (2008). Sense and sensitivity: How focus determines meaning. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Braun, B., & Tagliapietra, L. (2010). The role of contrastive intonation contours in the retrieval of contextual alternatives. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 1024–1043.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Byram-Washburn, M. (2013). Narrowing the Focus: Experimental Studies on Exhaustivity and Contrast. Ph.D. thesis, University of Southern California.Google Scholar
  6. Chemla, E., & Singh, R. (in press). Remarks on the experimental turn in the study of scalar implicature. Language and Linguistics Compass.Google Scholar
  7. Drenhaus, H., Zimmermann, M., & Vasishth, S. (2011). Exhaustiveness effects in clefts are not truth-functional. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24, 320–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Elmes, D. G., & Bjork, R. A. (1975). The interaction of encoding and rehearsal processes in the recall of repeated and nonrepeated items. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 30–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fox, D., & Katzir, R. (2011). On the characterization of alternatives. Natural Language Semantics, 19, 87–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fraundorf, S., Watson, D., & Benjamin, A. (2010). Recognition memory reveals just how contrastive contrastive accenting really is. Journal of Memory & Language, 63, 367–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gernsbacher, M., & Jescheniak, J. (1995). Cataphoric devices in spoken discourse. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 24–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Glenberg, A. M., Meyer, M., & Lindem, K. (1987). Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension. Journal of Memory & Language, 26, 69–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gotzner, N., & Spalek, K. (2014). Exhaustive inferences and additive presuppositions; the interplay of focus operators and contrastive intonation. In Proceedings of the European Summer School of Language, Logic and Computation.Google Scholar
  14. Gotzner, N., Wartenburger, I., & Spalek, K. (2016). The impact of focus particles on the recognition and rejection of contrastive alternatives. Language and Cognition, 8, 59–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Heister, J., Würzner, K. M., Bubenzer, J., Pohl, E., Hanneforth, T., Geyken, A., et al. (2011). Dlexdb - eine lexikalische Datenbank für die psychologische und linguistische Forschung. Psychologische Rundschau, 62, 10–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hermann, D. J., Mclaughlin, J. P., & Nelson, B. C. (1975). Visual and semantic factors in recognition from long-term memory. Memory & Cognition, 3, 381–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hofmeister, P. (2009). Encoding effects on memory retrieval in language comprehension. In Proceedings of 22nd CUNY Conference on Sentence Processing, Davis, CA.Google Scholar
  18. Husband, E. M., & Ferreira, F. (2016). The role of selection in the comprehension of focus alternatives. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31, 217–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Katzir, R. (2007). Structurally-defined alternatives. Linguistics and Philosophy, 30, 669–690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kim, C. (2012). Generating Alternatives: Interpreting Focus in Discourse. Ph.D. thesis, University of Rochester.Google Scholar
  21. MacDonald, M., & Just, M. (1989). Changes in activation levels with negation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 633–642.Google Scholar
  22. McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1980). The comprehension processes and memory structures involved in anaphoric reference. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 668–682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Molnár, V. (2002). Contrast-from a contrastive perspective. Language and Computers, 39, 147–161.Google Scholar
  24. Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Norris, D., Cutler, A., McQueen, J. M., & Butterfield, S. (2006). Phonological and conceptual activation in speech comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 53, 146–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Rooth, M. (1985). Association with Focus. Ph.D. thesis, Massachussets Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
  27. Rooth, M. (1992). A theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics, 1, 1–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Sturt, P., Sanford, A., Stewart, A., & Dawydiak, E. (2004). Linguistic focus and good-enough representations: An application of the change-detection paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 882–888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Swinney, D.A., Onifer, W., Prather, P., & Hirshkowitz, M. (1979). Semantic facilitation across sensory modalities in the processing of individual words and sentences. Memory & Cognition, 7, 159–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. van Casteren, M., & Davis, M. H. (2006). Mix, a program for pseudorandomization. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 584–589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nicole Gotzner
    • 1
  1. 1.Humboldt UniversityBerlinGermany

Personalised recommendations