Skip to main content

Infants and Children at the Cocktail Party

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Springer Handbook of Auditory Research ((SHAR,volume 60))

Abstract

The vast majority of children learn language despite the fact that they must do so in noisy environments. This chapter addresses the question of how children separate informative sounds from competing sounds and the limitations imposed on such auditory scene analysis by an immature auditory nervous system. Immature representation of auditory-visual synchrony, and possibly immature binaural processing, may limit the extent to which even school-age listeners can use those sources of information to parse the auditory scene. In contrast, infants have a relatively mature representation of sound spectrum, periodicity, and temporal modulation. Although infants and children are able to use these acoustic cues in auditory scene analysis, they are less efficient than adults at doing so. This lack of efficiency may stem from limitations of the mechanisms specifically involved in auditory scene analysis. However, the development of selective attention also makes an important contribution to the development of auditory scene analysis.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    A mismatch response is the difference between the response to a sound when it is presented frequently and the response to the same sound when it is the “oddball” in a sound sequence. In adults, the difference waveform is referred to as the mismatch negativity (MMN). However, the polarity of the response is positive in young infants, and many authors refer to the difference waveform as the mismatch response (MMR) when it is recorded in young infants. For simplicity, this response is referred to as the MMR throughout this chapter.

References

  • Abdala, C., & Folsom, R. C. (1995). Frequency contribution to the click-evoked auditory brain stem response in human adults and infants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 2394–2404.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abdala, C., & Keefe, D. H. (2012). Morphological and functional ear development. In L. A. Werner, A. N. Popper, & R. R. Fay (Eds.), Human auditory development (pp. 19–59). New York: Springer Science+Business Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akram, S., Englitz, B., Elhilali, M., Simon, J. Z., & Shamma, S. A. (2014). Investigating the neural correlates of a streaming percept in an informational-masking paradigm. PloS ONE, 9, e114427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alain, C., Theunissen, E. L., Chevalier, H., Batty, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2003). Developmental changes in distinguishing concurrent auditory objects. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 210–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, P., & Wightman, F. (1992). Spectral pattern discrimination by children. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 35, 222–233.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ashmead, D. H., Davis, D., Whalen, T., & Odom, R. (1991). Sound localization and sensitivity to interaural time differences in human infants. Child Development, 62, 1211–1226.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Assman, P. F., & Summerfield, Q. (1990). Modeling the perception of concurrent vowels: Vowels with different fundamental frequencies. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 88, 680–697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargones, J. Y., & Werner, L. A. (1994). Adults listen selectively; infants do not. Psychological Science, 5, 170–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartgis, J., Lilly, A. R., & Thomas, D. G. (2003). Event-related potential and behavioral measures of attention in 5-, 7-, and 9-year-olds. Journal of General Psychology, 130, 311–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bendor, D., & Wang, X. Q. (2010). Neural coding of periodicity in marmoset auditory cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 1809–1822.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berg, K. M., & Boswell, A. E. (1999). Effect of masker level on infants’ detection of tones in noise. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 61, 80–86.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bey, C., & McAdams, S. (2003). Postrecognition of interleaved melodies as an indirect measure of auditory stream formation. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 29, 267–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bolia, R. S., Nelson, W. T., Ericson, M. A., & Simpson, B. D. (2000). A speech corpus for multitalker communications research. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 107, 1065–1066.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bronkhorst, A. W. (2015). The cocktail-party problem revisited: Early processing and selection of multi-talker speech. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 77, 1465–1487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brungart, D. S. (2001). Informational and energetic masking effects in the perception of two simultaneous talkers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 1101–1109.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brungart, D. S., & Simpson, B. D. (2004). Within-ear and across-ear interference in a dichotic cocktail party listening task: Effects of masker uncertainty. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 115, 301–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2014). American time use survey. Retrieved from http://www.bls.gov/tus/

  • Buss, E., Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (2006). Development and the role of internal noise in detection and discrimination thresholds with narrow band stimuli. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 2777–2788.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, E., Hall, J. W., Grose, J. H., & Dev, M. B. (1999). Development of adult-like performance in backward, simultaneous, and forward masking. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 42, 844–849.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cabrera, L., Bertoncini, J., & Lorenzi, C. (2013). Perception of speech modulation cues by 6-month-old infants. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 56, 1733–1744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, S., & Dillon, H. (2007). Development of the listening in spatialized noise-sentences test (lisn-s). Ear and Hearing, 28, 196–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, K. A., & Small, S. A. (2014). Comparisons of auditory steady state response and behavioral air conduction and bone conduction thresholds for infants and adults with normal hearing. Ear and Hearing, 35, 423–439.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, R. S. (1981). Development of selective auditory attention skills in children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52, 379–385.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ching, T. Y. C., van Wanrooy, E., Dillon, H., & Carter, L. (2011). Spatial release from masking in normal-hearing children and children who use hearing aids. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129, 368–375.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, S., Lotto, A., Lewis, D., Hoover, B., & Stelmachowicz, P. (2008). Attentional modulation of word recognition by children in a dual-task paradigm. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 51, 1042–1054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarkson, M. G. (1992). Infants’ perception of low pitch. In L. A. Werner & E. W. Rubel (Eds.), Developmental psychoacoustics (pp. 159–188). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clarkson, M. G. (1996). Infants’ intensity discrimination; spectral profiles. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 181–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clohessy, A. B., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Development of the functional visual field. Acta Psychologica, 106, 51–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Culling, J. F., & Darwin, C. J. (1993). Perceptual separation of simultaneous vowels: Within and across-formant grouping by f0. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 93, 3454–3467.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cusack, R., & Roberts, B. (2000). Effects of differences in timbre on sequential grouping. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 62, 1112–1120.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. J., Brungart, D. S., & Simpson, B. D. (2003). Effects of fundamental frequency and vocal-tract length changes on attention to one of two simultaneous talkers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 114, 2913–2922.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. J., & Hukin, R. W. (1999). Auditory objects of attention: The role of interaural time differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology [Human Perception], 25, 617–629.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Demany, L. (1982). Auditory stream segregation in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 5, 261–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deroche, M. L., & Culling, J. F. (2011). Voice segregation by difference in fundamental frequency: Evidence for harmonic cancellation. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130, 2855–2865.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desjardins, R. N., Rogers, J., & Werker, J. F. (1997). An exploration of why preschoolers perform differently than do adults in audiovisual speech perception tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 85–110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desjardins, R. N., & Werker, J. F. (2004). Is the integration of heard and seen speech mandatory for infants? Developmental Psychobiology, 45, 187–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Doyle, A. B. (1973). Listening to distraction: A developmental study of selective attention. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 15, 100–115.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drullman, R., & Bronkhorst, A. W. (2004). Speech perception and talker segregation: Effects of level, pitch, and tactile support with multiple simultaneous talkers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116, 3090–3098.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egan, J. P. (1965). Masking-level differences as a function of interaural disparities in intensity of signal and of noise. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 38, 1043–1049.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eggermont, J. J., & Moore, J. K. (2012). Morphological and functional development of the auditory nervous system. In L. A. Werner, A. N. Popper, & R. R. Fay (Eds.), Human auditory development (pp. 61–105). New York: Springer Science+Business Media.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Eggermont, J. J., & Salamy, A. (1988). Maturational time course for the abr in preterm and full term infants. Hearing Research, 33, 35–48.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, L. L., Connors, S., Kille, E., Levin, S., et al. (1979). Children’s understanding of monosyllabic nouns in quiet and noise. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 66, 12–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fassbender, C. (1993). Auditory grouping and segregation processes in infancy. Norderstedt, Germany: Kaste Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant perception for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 279–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folland, N. A., Butler, B. E., Smith, N. A., & Trainor, L. J. (2012). Processing simultaneous auditory objects: Infants’ ability to detect mistuning in harmonic complexes. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131, 993–997.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folsom, R. C., & Wynne, M. K. (1987). Auditory brain stem responses from human adults and infants: Wave v tuning curves. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 81, 412–417.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gomes, H., Molholm, S., Ritter, W., Kurtzberg, D., et al. (2000). Mismatch negativity in children and adults, and effects of an attended task. Psychophysiology, 37, 807–816.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, K. W., & Seitz, P. F. (2000). The use of visible speech cues for improving auditory detection of spoken sentences. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 108, 1197–1208.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, K. W., Tufts, J. B., & Greenberg, S. (2007). Integration efficiency for speech perception within and across sensory modalities by normal-hearing and hearing-impaired individuals. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121, 1164–1176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grieco-Calub, T. M., & Litovsky, R. Y. (2012). Spatial acuity in 2- to 3-year-old children with normal acoustic hearing, unilateral cochlear implants, and bilateral cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing, 33, 561–572.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Grose, J. H., Hall, J. W., & Gibbs, C. (1993). Temporal analysis in children. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 36, 351–356.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gunther, T., Konrad, K., Haeusler, J., Saghraoui, H., et al. (2014). Developmental differences in visual and auditory attention: A cross-sectional study. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 25, 143–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. W., Buss, E., & Grose, J. H. (2005). Informational masking release in children and adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118, 1605–1613.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. W., Buss, E., & Grose, J. H. (2007). The binaural temporal window in adults and children. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121, 401–410.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. W., Buss, E., & Grose, J. H. (2014). Development of speech glimpsing in synchronously and asynchronously modulated noise. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 135, 3594–3600.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. W., Buss, E., Grose, J. H., & Dev, M. B. (2004). Developmental effects in the masking-level difference. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 47, 13–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1991). Notched-noise measures of frequency selectivity in adults and children using fixed-masker-level and fixed-signal-level presentation. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 34, 651–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1994). Development of temporal resolution in children as measured by the temporal-modulation transfer-function. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 96, 150–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. W., Grose, J. H., Buss, E., & Dev, M. B. (2002). Spondee recognition in a two-talker masker and a speech-shaped noise masker in adults and children. Ear and Hearing, 23, 159–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. W., Grose, J. H., & Dev, M. B. (1997). Auditory development in complex tasks of comodulation masking release. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40, 946–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. W., Haggard, M. P., & Fernandes, M. A. (1984). Detection in noise by spectro-temporal pattern analysis. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 76, 50–56.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, D. A., & Plack, C. J. (2009). Pitch processing sites in the human auditory brain. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 576–585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • He, C., Hotson, L., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Mismatch responses to pitch changes in early infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 878–892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • He, C., & Trainor, L. J. (2009). Finding the pitch of the missing fundamental in infants. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 7718–7722.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helfer, K. S., & Freyman, R. L. (2005). The role of visual speech cues in reducing energetic and informational masking. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 117, 842–849.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hillock-Dunn, A., & Wallace, M. T. (2012). Developmental changes in the multisensory temporal binding window persist into adolescence. Developmental Science, 15, 688–696.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hollich, G., Newman, R. S., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2005). Infants’ use of synchronized visual information to separate streams of speech. Child Development, 76, 598–613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holt, R. F., Kirk, K. I., & Hay-McCutcheon, M. (2011). Assessing multimodal spoken word-in-sentence recognition in children with normal hearing and children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 54, 632–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, D., Werner, L. A., Rubinstein, J., & Won, J. H. (2013). Spectral ripple discrimination in infants: Effect of ripple depth and envelope phase randomization. Abstracts of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, 37, 601.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, P. M., & Litovsky, R. Y. (2006). Effect of masker type and age on speech intelligibility and spatial release from masking in children and adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 2177–2189.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, P. R., Moore, D. R., & Amitay, S. (2015). Development of auditory selective attention: Why children struggle to hear in noisy environments. Developmental Psychology, 51, 353–369.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Karns, C. M., Isbell, E., Giuliano, R. J., & Neville, H. J. (2015). Auditory attention in childhood and adolescence: An event-related potential study of spatial selective attention to one of two simultaneous stories. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 53–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, G., Mason, C. R., Deliwala, P. S., Woods, W. S., & Colburn, H. S. (1994). Reducing informational masking by sound segregation. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95, 3475–3480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, G., Mason, C. R., & Richards, V. M. (2003). Multiple bursts, multiple looks, and stream coherence in the release from informational masking. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 114, 2835–2845.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kopp, F. (2014). Audiovisual temporal fusion in 6-month-old infants. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 56–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kopp, F., & Dietrich, C. (2013). Neural dynamics of audiovisual synchrony and asynchrony perception in 6-month-old infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1982). The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science, 218, 1138–1140.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhnle, S., Ludwig, A. A., Meuret, S., Kuttner, C., et al. (2013). Development of auditory localization accuracy and auditory spatial discrimination in children and adolescents. Audiology and Neurotology, 18, 48–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lalonde, K., & Holt, R. F. (2015). Preschoolers benefit from visually salient speech cues. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 58, 135–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, B. K., & Werner, L. A. (2012). Perception of missing fundamental pitch by 3- and 4-month-old human infants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132, 3874–3882.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, B. K., & Werner, L. A. (2014). Perception of the pitch of unresolved harmonics by 3-and 7-month-old human infants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 136, 760–767.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leibold, L. J. (2012). Development of auditory scene analysis and auditory attention. In L. A. Werner, A. N. Popper, & R. R. Fay (Eds.), Human auditory development (pp. 137–161). New York: Springer Science+Business Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leibold, L. J., & Bonino, A. Y. (2009). Release from informational masking in children: Effect of multiple signal bursts. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125, 2200–2208.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leibold, L. J., & Buss, E. (2013). Children’s identification of consonants in a speech-shaped noise or a two-talker masker. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 56, 1144–1155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leibold, L. J., & Neff, D. L. (2007). Effects of masker-spectral variability and masker fringes in children and adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121, 3666–3676.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leibold, L. J., & Neff, D. L. (2011). Masking by a remote-frequency noise band in children and adults. Ear and Hearing, 32, 663–666.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leibold, L. J., & Werner, L. A. (2007). The effect of masker-frequency variability on the detection performance of infants and adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 3960–3970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewkowicz, D. J. (1992). Infants’ responsiveness to the auditory and visual attributes of a sounding/moving stimulus. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 52, 519–528.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lewkowicz, D. J. (1996). Perception of auditory-visual temporal synchrony in human infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1094–1106.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewkowicz, D. J. (2010). Infant perception of audio-visual speech synchrony. Developmental Psychology, 46, 66–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewkowicz, D. J., & Flom, R. (2014). The audiovisual temporal binding window narrows in early childhood. Child Development, 85, 685–694.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Litovsky, R. Y. (1997). Developmental changes in the precedence effect: Estimates of minimum audible angle. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 1739–1745.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Litovsky, R. Y. (2005). Speech intelligibility and spatial release from masking in young children. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 117, 3091–3099.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Litovsky, R. (2012). Development of binaural and spatial hearing. In L. A. Werner, A. N. Popper, & R. R. Fay (Eds.), Human auditory development (pp. 163–195). New York: Springer Science+Business Media.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • MacKain, K., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Spieker, S., & Stern, D. (1983). Infant intermodal speech perception is a left-hemisphere function. Science, 219, 1347–1349.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Massaro, D. W. (1984). Children’s perception of visual and auditory speech. Child Development, 55, 1777–1788.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Massaro, D. W. (1998). Perceiving talking faces: From speech perception to a behavioral principle. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massaro, D. W., Thompson, L. A., Barron, B., & Laren, E. (1986). Developmental-changes in visual and auditory contributions to speech-perception. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 93–113.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, S., & Bertoncini, J. (1997). Organization and discrimination of repeating sound sequences by newborn infants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 2945–2953.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746–748.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Micheyl, C., Kreft, H., Shamma, S., & Oxenham, A. J. (2013). Temporal coherence versus harmonicity in auditory stream formation. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 133, EL188–EL194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Middlebrooks, J. C., Nick, H. S., Subramony, S. H., Advincula, J., et al. (2013). Mutation in the kv3.3 voltage-gated potassium channel causing spinocerebellar ataxia 13 disrupts sound-localization mechanisms. PLoS ONE, 8, 6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. R., Cowan, J. A., Riley, A., Edmondson-Jones, A. M., & Ferguson, M. A. (2011). Development of auditory processing in 6- to 11-yr-old children. Ear and Hearing, 32, 269–285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, J., Summerfield, A. Q., O’Donoghuea’c, G. M., & Moore, D. R. (2011). Spatial hearing of normally hearing and cochlear implanted children. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 75, 489–494.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Neff, D. L. (1995). Signal properties that reduce masking by simultaneous, random-frequency maskers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98, 1909–1920.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neff, D. L., & Green, D. M. (1987). Masking produced by spectral uncertainty with multicomponent maskers. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 41, 409–415.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, R. S. (2009). Infants’ listening in multitalker environments: Effect of the number of background talkers. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 822–836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, R. S., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1996). The cocktail party effect in infants. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 58, 1145–1156.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nozza, R. J. (1987). The binaural masking level difference in infants and adults: Developmental change in binaural hearing. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 105–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nozza, R. J., Wagner, E. F., & Crandell, M. A. (1988). Binaural release from masking for a speech sound in infants, preschoolers, and adults. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 31, 212–218.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Oh, E. L., Wightman, F., & Lutfi, R. A. (2001). Children’s detection of pure-tone signals with random multitone maskers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 2888–2895.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Olsho, L. W. (1985). Infant auditory perception: Tonal masking. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 371–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L., & Werker, J. F. (2003). Two-month-old infants match phonetic information in lips and voice. Developmental Science, 6, 191–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peter, V., Wong, K., Narne, V. K., Sharma, M., et al. (2014). Assessing spectral and temporal processing in children and adults using temporal modulation transfer function (TMTF), iterated ripple noise (IRN) perception, and spectral ripple discrimination (SRD). Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 25, 210–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35(35), 73–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ponton, C. W., Moore, J. K., & Eggermont, J. J. (1996). Auditory brain stem response generation by parallel pathways: Differential maturation of axonal conduction time and synaptic transmission. Ear and Hearing, 17, 402–410.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prodi, N., Visentin, C., & Feletti, A. (2013). On the perception of speech in primary school classrooms: Ranking of noise interference and of age influence. Journal of the Acoustical Societ of America, 133, 255–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rahne, T., Bochmann, M., von Specht, H., & Sussman, E. S. (2007). Visual cues can modulate integration and segregation of objects in auditory scene analysis. Brain Research, 1144, 127–135.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rahne, T., & Bockmann-Barthel, M. (2009). Visual cues release the temporal coherence of auditory objects in auditory scene analysis. Brain Research, 1300, 125–134.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L. A., Molholm, S., Blanco, D., Gomez-Ramirez, M., et al. (2011). The development of multisensory speech perception continues into the late childhood years. European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 2329–2337.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Developing mechanisms temperamental effortful control. Journal of Personality, 71, 1113–1143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2011). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation in early life. Emotion Review, 3, 207–213.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., et al. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1029–1040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, L. D., Stevens, C., Coch, D., & Neville, H. J. (2006). Selective auditory attention in 3- to 5-year-old children: An event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2126–2138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, B. A., Bull, D., & Trehub, S. E. (1988). Binaural unmasking in infants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 83, 1124–1132.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, B. A., Morrongiello, B. A., & Trehub, S. E. (1990). The size of the critical band in infants, children, and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 16, 642–652.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, B. A., Trehub, S. E., Morrongiello, B. A., & Thorpe, L. A. (1989). Developmental changes in masked thresholds. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 86, 1733–1742.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shamma, S., Elhilali, M., Ma, L., Micheyl, C., et al. (2013). Temporal coherence and the streaming of complex sounds. In B. C. J. Moore, R. D. Patterson, I. M. Winter, R. P. Carlyon, & H. E. Gockel (Eds.), Basic aspects of hearing: Physiology and perception (Vol. 787, pp. 535–543). New York: Springer Science+Business Media.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Shamma, S. A., Elhilali, M., & Micheyl, C. (2011). Temporal coherence and attention in auditory scene analysis. Trends in Neurosciences, 34, 114–123.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siqueland, E. R., & Delucia, C. A. (1969). Visual reinforcement of nonnutritive sucking in human infants. Science, 165, 1144.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, N. A., & Trainor, L. J. (2011). Auditory stream segregation improves infants’ selective attention to target tones amid distractors. Infancy, 16, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spetner, N. B., & Olsho, L. W. (1990). Auditory frequency resolution in human infancy. Child Development, 61, 632–652.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, A. (2008). Reception thresholds for sentences in quiet, continuous noise, and interrupted noise in school-age children. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 19, 135–146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sumby, W. H., & Pollack, I. (1954). Visual contribution to speech intelligibility in noise. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 26, 212–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Summerfield, Q., & Assmann, P. F. (1991). Perception of concurrent vowels: Effects of harmonic misalignment and pitch-period asynchrony. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89, 1364–1377.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sussman, E. S., Ceponiene, R., Shestakova, A., Naatanen, R., & Winkler, I. (2001). Auditory stream segregation processes operate similarly in school-aged children and adults. Hearing Research, 153, 108–114.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sussman, E. S., & Steinschneider, M. (2009). Attention effects on auditory scene analysis in children. Neuropsychologia, 47, 771–785.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sussman, E. S., Wong, R., Horvath, J., Winkler, I., & Wang, W. (2007). The development of the perceptual organization of sound by frequency separation in 5–11-year-old children. Hearing Research, 225, 117–127.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Truchon-Gagnon, C. (1988). Noise in day-care centers for children. Noise Control Engineering Journal, 39, 57–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaillancourt, V., Laroche, C., Giguere, C., & Soil, S. D. (2008). Establishment of age-specific normative data for the Canadian French version of the hearing in noise test for children. Ear and Hearing, 29, 453–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Deun, L., van Wieringen, A., Van den Bogaert, T., Scherf, F., et al. (2009). Sound localization, sound lateralization, and binaural masking level differences in young children with normal hearing. Ear and Hearing, 30, 178–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Noorden, L. P. A. S. (1975). Temporal coherence in the perception of tone sequences. PhD dissertation, Eindhoven University of Technology, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, L. A. (2006a). Amplitude modulation detection by infants and adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 3234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, L. A. (2006b). Preliminary observations on the temporal modulation transfer functions of infants and adults. Abstracts of the American Auditory Society Annual Meeting.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, L. A. (2013). Infants’ detection and discrimination of sounds in modulated maskers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 133, 4156–4167.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, L. A., & Bargones, J. Y. (1991). Sources of auditory masking in infants: Distraction effects. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 50, 405–412.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, L. A., & Leibold, L. J. (2017). Auditory development in normal-hearing children. In R. Sewald & A. M. Tharpe (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of pediatric audiology (2nd ed., pp. 67–86). New York: Plural Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, L. A., Marean, G. C., Halpin, C. F., Spetner, N. B., & Gillenwater, J. M. (1992). Infant auditory temporal acuity: Gap detection. Child Development, 63, 260–272.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, L. A., Parrish, H. K., & Holmer, N. M. (2009). Effects of temporal uncertainty and temporal expectancy on infants’ auditory sensitivity. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125, 1040–1049.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wightman, F., Allen, P., Dolan, T., Kistler, D., & Jamieson, D. (1989). Temporal resolution in children. Child Development, 60, 611–624.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wightman, F., Callahan, M. R., Lutfi, R. A., Kistler, D. J., & Oh, E. (2003). Children’s detection of pure-tone signals: Informational masking with contralateral maskers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113, 3297–3305.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wightman, F., & Kistler, D. J. (2005). Informational masking of speech in children: Effects of ipsilateral and contralateral distracters. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118, 3164–3176.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wightman, F., Kistler, D., & Brungart, D. (2006). Informational masking of speech in children: Auditory-visual integration. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 3940–3949.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Winkler, I., Kushnerenko, E., Horvath, J., Ceponiene, R., et al. (2003). Newborn infants can organize the auditory world. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 100, 11812–11815.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, B. A., & Fitzgerald, M. B. (2001). Different patterns of human discrimination learning for two interaural cues to sound-source location. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 98, 12307–12312.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yuen, K. C. P., & Yuan, M. (2014). Development of spatial release from masking in mandarin-speaking children with normal hearing. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 57, 2005–2023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zettler, C. M., Sevcik, R. A., Morris, R. D., & Clarkson, M. G. (2008). Comodulation masking release (CMR) in children and the influence of reading status. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 51, 772–784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lynne Werner .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Werner, L. (2017). Infants and Children at the Cocktail Party. In: Middlebrooks, J., Simon, J., Popper, A., Fay, R. (eds) The Auditory System at the Cocktail Party. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research, vol 60. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-51662-2_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics