Skip to main content

The Cognitive Ecology of the Internet

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Cognition Beyond the Brain

Abstract

In this chapter, we analyze the relationships between the Internet and its users in terms of situated cognition theory. We first argue that the Internet is a new kind of cognitive ecology , providing almost constant access to a vast amount of digital information that is increasingly more integrated into our cognitive routines. We then briefly introduce situated cognition theory and its species of embedded, embodied, extended, distributed and collective cognition. Having thus set the stage, we begin by taking an embedded cognition view and analyze how the Internet aids certain cognitive tasks. After that, we conceptualize how the Internet enables new kinds of embodied interaction , extends certain aspects of our embodiment, and examine how wearable technologies that monitor physiological, behavioral and contextual states transform the embodied self. On the basis of the degree of cognitive integration between a user and Internet resource, we then look at how and when the Internet extends our cognitive processes. We end this chapter with a discussion of distributed and collective cognition as facilitated by the Internet.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The current chapter uses the term ‘Internet’ as a catch-all term for all the various applications that are built on top of the Internet. This includes the World Wide Web, which is currently the most popular Internet application. As such, when we refer to the Internet as a cognitive ecology, we mean to suggest that the Web (as well as all other Internet applications, such as email) should be included as part of the cognitive ecology.

  2. 2.

    Smart (2013), for example, suggests that the Web provides a new kind of ecological context in which advanced forms of machine intelligence might emerge.

  3. 3.

    This represents a subset of all the areas that could have been listed. Other areas of notable interest from a cognitive science perspective include cloud computing (see Clowes 2015), the Semantic Web (see Smart, in press), and the Internet of Things (see Sect. Embedded Cognition).

  4. 4.

    In particular, the use of linked data formats helps to separate issues of information presentation from issues of information representation. This kind of ‘presentational agnosticism’ is crucial when it comes to the flexible (and dynamic) creation of cues, prompts, and affordances that serve to shape the profile of human thought and action (see Smart, in press).

  5. 5.

    It is also interesting to note the way in which this ubiquitous and ever-present ‘data environment’ helps to provide new opportunities for the implementation of location-aware intelligent systems, such as driverless cars and aerial drones.

  6. 6.

    Aside from its cognitive and epistemic effects, the Internet also influences the structure and organization of social processes. This raises a wealth of (socio-economic, socio-cultural and socio-political) issues that are the current focus of attention within the social sciences and digital humanities (Lupton 2015; van Dijk 2012). An in-depth discussion of such issues is beyond the scope of the current chapter; however, it is important to bear in mind that the Internet may sometimes be seen to exert an indirect influence on cognitive processes as a result of its ability to reshape the wider social, political, cultural and economic landscapes in which much of human thought and action takes place.

  7. 7.

    Whilst this is a helpful tripartite distinction between the species of situated cognition theory, it is not exhaustive. There are other views such as enactivism (Stewart et al. 2010), collective cognition (Smart et al. 2010b) and transactive memory theory (Wegner 1995) that are also non-Cartesian in that they are concerned with the way a cognitive agent is situated in the environment.

  8. 8.

    Note that inasmuch as we see prospective memory as a form of memory in which we perform future actions without explicit instructions (see Baddeley et al. 2009), it is unclear to what extent we should regard reminder systems as implementing a form of (external) prospective memory.

  9. 9.

    This notion of a ‘virtual designer environment’ builds on the notion of a ‘designer environment’ as discussed by Clark (1997): “We build ‘designer environments’ in which human reason is able to far outstrip the computational ambit of the unaugmented biological brain” (Clark 1997, p. 191).

  10. 10.

    Importantly, issues of embodiment often surface in the context of research into cognitive robotics (see Pfeifer and Bongard 2007). This highlights the importance of a non-biological conception of the body to embodied cognitive science: in the absence of such a conception it becomes difficult to adopt a unified perspective of research into a rich variety of materially-diverse (e.g., biological, robotic and virtual) embodied cognitive systems (see Smart and Sycara 2015).

  11. 11.

    The thing that is important to remember, here, is that inasmuch as a non-biological resource counts as part of an organism’s body, then (relative to the claims made by proponents of embodied cognition) the resource is (potentially) poised to play a role in shaping that organism’s cognitive processing routines. As a result, if an Internet-enabled device counts as a part of the body (on the basis of functional criteria), then it seems that it should be just as much a focus of analytic attention for the proponent of embodied cognition as should a more conventional (i.e., biological) body part.

  12. 12.

    A similar point is made by Kunze et al. (2013). They suggest that the use of mobile sensing technologies portends an era in which technology is able to recognize and monitor various forms of cognitive activity, revolutionizing our understanding of the factors that contribute to optimal cognitive performance, as well as providing new ways for technology to shape and scaffold our cognitive routines.

  13. 13.

    For example, personal data could be used to evaluate hypotheses concerning the links between sleep patterns and the etiologic bases of diabetes (Tasali et al. 2008), depression (Landsness et al. 2011) and dementia (Sharma et al. 2015).

  14. 14.

    This is sometimes referred to as para-synthetic expression (Won et al. 2015).

  15. 15.

    Explicit access to physiological information (e.g., heart rate) can also, on occasion, influence our sense of body ownership concerning a non-biological appendage (see Suzuki et al. 2013).

  16. 16.

    Problems with trust often lie at the root of these concerns. Clark (2010), for example, claims people do not trust online content to the same extent that they trust information retrieved from bio-memory. From an empirical perspective, however, it is far from clear that people really do subject online information to the sort of evaluative scrutiny that would undermine its candidacy for cognitive incorporation (see Smart, in press). In addition, there a variety of reasons to suspect that at least some sources of online content can be implicitly trusted. Individuals may, for example, rely on the use of cloud-based personal data stores (see Van Kleek and O’Hara 2014) as a source of trusted information. They may also exploit a range of so-called ‘online reliability indicators’ (Smart and Shadbolt, in press) to guide metacognitive processes relating to information selection and endorsement (Arango-Muñoz 2013). Interestingly, processes that give rise to these indicators can, on occasion, be cast as a form of collective or distributed cognition. Ben-Naim et al. (2013), for example, present a distributed approach to the construction of (social) trust metrics, which are subsequently used to guide decisions relating to the endorsement of expert recommendations.

  17. 17.

    Virtual team-working is a form of team-working that relies on the use of information and communications technology to support task-relevant forms of information exchange, information processing and inter-agent coordination (see Powell et al. 2004).

  18. 18.

    Although it is easy to see such forms of processing as a relatively recent phenomenon, it is important to remember that technologically low-grade variants of socio-computational processing date back to at least the 18th century (see Grier 2013).

  19. 19.

    A virtual team, in this case, is simply a collection of individuals that engages in a form of virtual team-working (see Powell et al. 2004). Crucially, nothing in this definition rules out the possibility that a virtual team could (at different points in time) also function as a real-world (or face-to-face) team. The result is that any form of (conventional) team cognition is also (potentially at least) a form of virtual team cognition. Consider, for example, how collaborative sensemaking technologies (e.g., Shrager et al. 2010; Toniolo et al. 2014) might be used to support the kinds of analyses undertaken by (e.g.) criminal investigators (Baber 2013).

  20. 20.

    A human computation system, recall, is a system that combines human and machine capabilities to perform complex computational tasks (Law and von Ahn 2011; Michelucci 2013; Quinn and Bederson 2011).

  21. 21.

    In the case of Foldit, human pattern matching and spatial reasoning abilities are used to help solve the problem of predicting the three-dimensional structure of selected proteins (Khatib et al. 2011). Galaxy Zoo, in contrast, relies on human visual pattern recognition to detect and classify galaxies from large-scale astronomical image databases (Lintott et al. 2008).

References

  • Aglioti, S., Smania, N., Manfredi, M., & Berlucchi, G. (1996). Disownership of left hand and objects related to it in a patient with right brain damage. NeuroReport, 8(1), 293–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. (2003). Embodied cognition: A field guide. Artificial Intelligence, 149(1), 91–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arango-Muñoz, S. (2013). Scaffolded memory and metacognitive feelings. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(1), 135–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baber, C. (2013). Distributed cognition at the crime scene. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Computation, interactivity and human artifice. London, UK: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A., Eysenck, M. W., & Anderson, M. C. (2009). Memory. Hove, East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. London, UK: Jason Aronson Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Naim, J., Bonnefon, J. F., Herzig, A., Leblois, S., & Lorini, E. (2013). Computer-mediated trust in self-interested expert recommendations. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Computation, interactivity and human artifice. London, UK: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biocca, F. (1999). The cyborg’s dilemma: Progressive embodiment in virtual environments. In J. P. Marsh, B. Gorayska, & J. L. Mey (Eds.), Humane interfaces: Questions of method and practice in cognitive technology. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonabeau, E. (2009). Decisions 2.0: The power of collective intelligence. MIT Sloan Management Review, 50(2), 45–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, N. (2010). The shallows: How the internet is changing the way we think, read and remember. London, UK: Atlantic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, E. H. (2008). The social web: Research and opportunities. Computer, 41(9), 88–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, E. H. (2009). Augmented social cognition: Using social web technology to enhance the ability of groups to remember, think, and reason. In 35th SIGMOD International Conference on Management of Data, Providence, Rhode Island, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, E. H., Pirolli, P., Suh, B., Kittur, A., Pendleton, B., Mytkowicz, T. (2008). Augmented social cognition. In AAAI Spring Symposium on Social Information Processing, Stanford, California, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body and world together again. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1999). Where brain, body, and world collide. Cognitive Systems Research, 1(1), 5–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2001). Mindware: An introduction to the philosophy of cognitive Science. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2007). Re-inventing ourselves: The plasticity of embodiment, sensing, and mind. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 32(3), 263–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. New York, New York, USA: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2010). Memento’s revenge: The extended mind, extended. In R. Menary (Ed.), The extended mind. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2011). Finding the mind. Philosophical Studies, 152(3), 447–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 7–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clowes, R. (2015). Thinking in the cloud: The cognitive incorporation of cloud-based technology. Philosophy & Technology, 28(2), 261–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clowes, R. W. (2013). The cognitive integration of e-memory. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(1), 107–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooke, N. J., Gorman, J. C., Winner, J. L. (2007). Team cognition. In F. T. Durso, R. S. Nickerson, S. T. Dumais, S. Lewandowsky, T. J. Perfect (Eds.), Handbook of applied cognition (2nd edn). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Dijk, J. (2012). The network society (3rd ed.). London, UK: Sage Publications Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (1993). Precis of origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(4), 737–791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dourish, P. (2001). Where the action is: The foundations of embodied interaction. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drain, C., & Strong, R. C. (2015). Situated mediation and technological reflexivity: Smartphones, extended memory, and limits of cognitive enhancement. In F. Scalambrino (Ed.), Social epistemology and technology: Toward public self-awareness regarding technological mediation. Lanham, Maryland, USA: Rowman & Littlefield International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fallis, D. (2011). Wikipistemology. In A. I. Goldman & D. Whitcomb (Eds.), Social epistemology: Essential readings. New York, USA: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Floridi, L. (2011). The construction of personal identities online. Minds and Machines, 21(4), 477–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Floridi, L. (2014). The fourth revolution: How the infosphere is reshaping human reality. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gant, D., & Kiesler, S. (2001). Blurring the boundaries: Cell phones, mobility, and the line between work and personal life. In B. Brown, N. Green, & R. Harper (Eds.), Wireless world: Social and interactional aspects of the mobile age. London, UK: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gemmell, J., & Bell, G. (2009). The e-memory revolution. Library Journal, 134(15), 20–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gemmell, J., Bell, G., & Lueder, R. (2006). MyLifeBits: A personal database for everything. Communications of the ACM, 49(1), 88–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R. N. (2007). Distributed cognition without distributed knowing. Social Epistemology, 21(3), 313–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R. N. (2012). Scientific cognition: Human centered but not human bound. Philosophical Explorations, 15(2), 199–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greengard, S. (2015). The internet of things. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, R. L. (1993). Mind in science: A history of explanations in psychology and physics. London, UK: Penguin Books Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grier, D. A. (2013). Human computation and divided labour: The precursors of modern crowdsourcing. In P. Michelucci (Ed.), Handbook of human computation. New York, USA: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpin, H. (2013). Does the web extend the mind? In 5th Annual ACM Web Science Conference, Paris, France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpin, H., Clark, A., Wheeler, M. (2010). Towards a philosophy of the web: Representation, enaction, collective intelligence. In Web Science Conference, Raleigh, North Carolina, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heersmink, R. (2015). Dimensions of integration in embedded and extended cognitive systems. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 14(3), 577–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heersmink, R. (2016). The cognitive integration of scientific instruments: Information, situated cognition and scientific practice. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 517-537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heintz, C. (2006). Web search engines and distributed assessment systems. Pragmatics & Cognition, 14(2), 387–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helmstaedter, M., Briggman, K. L., Turaga, S. C., Jain, V., Seung, H. S., & Denk, W. (2013). Connectomic reconstruction of the inner plexiform layer in the mouse retina. Nature, 500(7461), 168–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hendler, J., & Berners-Lee, T. (2010). From the semantic web to social machines: A research challenge for AI on the world wide web. Artificial Intelligence, 174, 156–161.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Heylighen, F. (2013). From human computation to the global brain: The self-organization of distributed intelligence. In P. Michelucci (Ed.), Handbook of human computation. New York, USA: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2010). Cognitive ecology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(4), 705–715.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2011). Enculturating the supersized mind. Philosophical Studies, 152(3), 437–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2014). The cultural ecosystem of human cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 27(1), 34–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janssen, J. H., Bailenson, J. N., Ijsselsteijn, W., & Westerink, J. H. (2010). Intimate heartbeats: Opportunities for affective communication technology. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 1(2), 72–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanawattanachai, P., & Yoo, Y. (2002). Dynamic nature of trust in virtual teams. The Journal of Strategic Information Systems, 11(3), 187–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kearns, M. (2012). Experiments in social computation. Communications of the ACM, 55(10), 56–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khatib, F., Cooper, S., Tyka, M.D., Xu, K., Makedon, I., Popovic, Z., Baker, D., Foldit Players (2011). Algorithm discovery by protein folding game players. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(47):18, 949–18, 953.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18, 513–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Nussinson, R. (2009). Attributing study effort to data-driven and goal-driven effects: Implications for metacognitive judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(5), 1338–1343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraut, R., Maher, M. L., Olson, J., Malone, T. W., Pirolli, P., & Thomas, J. C. (2010). Scientific foundations: A case for technology-mediated social-participation theory. Computer, 43(11), 22–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunze, K., Iwamura, M., Kise, K., Uchida, S., & Omachi, S. (2013). Activity recognition for the mind: Toward a cognitive “quantified self”. Computer, 46(10), 105–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landsness, E. C., Goldstein, M. R., Peterson, M. J., Tononi, G., & Benca, R. M. (2011). Antidepressant effects of selective slow wave sleep deprivation in major depression: A high-density EEG investigation. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(8), 1019–1026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Law, E., & von Ahn, L. (2011). Human computation. Synthesis Lectures on Artificial Intelligence and Machine Learning, 5(3), 1–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lintott, C. J., & Reed, J. (2013). Human computation in citizen science. In P. Michelucci (Ed.), Handbook of human computation. New York, USA: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lintott, C. J., Schawinski, K., Slosar, A., Land, K., Bamford, S., Thomas, D., et al. (2008). Galaxy Zoo: Morphologies derived from visual inspection of galaxies from the sloan digital sky survey. Monthly Notices of the Royal Astronomical Society, 389(3), 1179–1189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ludwig, D. (2015). Extended cognition and the explosion of knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 28(3), 355–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lupton, D. (2013). Understanding the human machine. IEEE Technology and Society Magazine, 32(4), 25–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lupton, D. (2015). Digital sociology. Abingdon, Oxon, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M. P. (2014). Neuromedia, extended knowledge and understanding. Philosophical Issues, 24(1), 299–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(8), 4398–4403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2004). The cognitive basis of material engagement: Where brain, body and culture conflate. In E. DeMarrais, C. Gosden, & C. Renfrew (Eds.), Rethinking materiality: The engagement of mind with the material world. Cambridge, UK: The McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malone, T. W., Laubacher, R., & Dellarocas, C. (2010). The collective intelligence genome. MIT Sloan Management Review, 51(3), 21–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marx, V. (2013). Neuroscience waves to the crowd. Nature Methods, 10(11), 1069–1074.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, W. (2013). Collective search as human computation. In P. Michelucci (Ed.), Handbook of human computation. New York, USA: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, W., & Watts, D. J. (2012). Collaborative learning in networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(3), 764–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, W. A., Jones, A., & Goldstone, R. L. (2008). Propagation of innovations in networked groups. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(3), 422–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, T., Hashimoto, S., & Okude, N. (2008). The embodied web: Embodied web-services interaction with an umbrella for augmented city experiences. Computer Animation and Virtual Worlds, 19(1), 49–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer-Schönberger, V. (2011). Delete: The virtue of forgetting in the digital age. Princeton, New Jersey, USA: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBeath, M., Shaffer, D., & Kaiser, M. (1995). How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls. Science, 268(5210), 569–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. (2010). Dimensions of mind. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 561–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michaelian, K., & Arango-Muñoz, S. (2014). Epistemic feelings, epistemic emotions: Review and introduction to the focus section. Philosophical Inquiries, 2(1), 97–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michelucci, P. (Ed.). (2013). Handbook of human computation. New York, USA: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michelucci, P. (2016). Human computation and convergence. In W. Bainbridge & M. Roco (Eds.), Handbook of science and technology convergence. New York, USA: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. (2014). The fourth screen: Mediatization and the smartphone. Mobile Media & Communication, 2(2), 209–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M. (2015). The internet of things: How smart TVs, smart cars, smart homes, and smart cities are changing the world. Indianapolis, Indiana, USA: Que Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (2009). How do engineering scientists think? Model-based simulation in biomedical engineering research laboratories. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(4), 730–757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, M., & Cowley, S. J. (2013). Human agency and the resources of reason. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Computation, interactivity and human artifice. London, UK: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oksman, V., & Rautianen, P. (2003a). Extension of the hand: Children’s and teenagers’ relationship with the mobile phone in Finland. In L. Fortunati, J. Katz, & R. Riccini (Eds.), Mediating the human body: Technology, communication and fashion. Mahwah, New Jersey, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oksman, V., & Rautianen, P. (2003b). “Perhaps it is a body part”: How the mobile phone became an organic part of the everyday lives of finnish children and teenagers. In J. Katz (Ed.), Machines that become us: The social context of communication technology. New Brunswick, New Jersey, USA: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parameswaran, M., & Whinston, A. B. (2007). Research issues in social computing. Journal of the Association for Information Systems, 8(6), 336–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfeifer, R., & Bongard, J. (2007). How the body shapes the way we think: A new view of intelligence. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, A., Piccoli, G., & Ives, B. (2004). Virtual teams: A review of current literature and directions for future research. The DATA BASE for Advances in Information Systems, 35(1), 6–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, A., Bederson, B. (2011). Human computation: A survey and taxonomy of a growing field. In Annual Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI’11), Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, P., & Aydede, M. (2009). A short primer on situated cognition. In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rupert, R. (2004). Challenges to the hypothesis of extended cognition. Journal of Philosophy, 101(8), 389–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salas, E., Fiore, S. M., & Letsky, M. P. (Eds.). (2011). Theories of team cognition: Cross-disciplinary perspectives. New York, New York, USA: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salerno, S., Zamagni, E., Urquizar, C., Salemme, R., Farn, A., & Frassinetti, F. (2012). Increases of corticospinal excitability in self-related processing. European Journal of Neuroscience, 36(5), 2716–2721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, L. (2007). The embodied cognition research programme. Philosophy Compass, 2(2), 338–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, L. A. (2011). Embodied cognition. Abingdon, Oxon, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharma, V. K., Sharma, P., Deshmukh, R., & Singh, R. (2015). Age associated sleep loss: A trigger for Alzheimer’s disease. Bulletin of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 25(1), 78–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrager, J., Billman, D., Convertino, G., Massar, J. P., & Pirolli, P. (2010). Soccer science and the Bayes community: Exploring the cognitive implications of modern scientific communication. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(1), 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, T. W. (2012). Evaluating Google as an epistemic tool. Metaphilosophy, 43(4), 426–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smart, P. R. (2012). The web-extended mind. Metaphilosophy, 43(4), 426–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smart, P. R. (2013). Understanding the cognitive impact of emerging Web technologies: A research focus area for embodied, extended and distributed approaches to cognition. In 1st International Web for Wellbeing & Human Performance Workshop, Paris, France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smart, P. R. (2014). Embodiment, cognition and the world wide web. In L. A. Shapiro (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition. New York, USA: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smart, P. R. (in press). Emerging digital technologies: Implications for extended conceptions of cognition and knowledge. In: Carter, A. J., Clark, A., Kallestrup, J., Palermos, O. S., Pritchard, D. (Eds.), Extended epistemology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smart, P. R., Shadbolt, N. R. (in press). The world wide web. In: J. Chase, D. Coady (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of applied epistemology. New York, USA: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smart, P. R., Sycara, K. (2015). Situating cognition in the virtual world. In 6th International Conference on Applied Human Factors and Ergonomics, Las Vegas, Nevada, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smart, P. R., Huynh, T. D., Braines, D., Shadbolt, N. R. (2010a), Dynamic networks and distributed problem-solving. In Knowledge Systems for Coalition Operations (KSCO’10), Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smart, P. R., Huynh, T. D., Braines, D., Sycara, K., Shadbolt, N. R. (2010b) Collective cognition: Exploring the dynamics of belief propagation and collective problem solving in multi-agent systems. In 1st ITA Workshop on Network-Enabled Cognition: The Contribution of Social and Technological Networks to Human Cognition, Maryland, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smart, P. R., Simperl, E., & Shadbolt, N. R. (2014). A taxonomic framework for social machines. In D. Miorandi, V. Maltese, M. Rovatsos, A. Nijholt, & J. Stewart (Eds.), Social collective intelligence: Combining the powers of humans and machines to build a smarter society. Berlin, Germany: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, B., Liu, J., & Wegner, D. (2011). Google effects on memory: Cognitive consequences of having information at our fingertips. Science, 333(6043), 776–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staley, D. J. (2014). Brain, mind and internet: A deep history and future. Basingstoke, England, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a hostile world: The evolution of human cognition. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2010). Minds: Extended or scaffolded? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 465–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, J. R., Gapenne, O., & Di Paolo, E. A. (2010). Enaction: Toward a new paradigm for cognitive science. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J. (2006). Distributed cognition: Domains and dimensions. Pragmatics & Cognition, 14(2), 235–247.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J. (2010). Exograms and interdisciplinarity: History, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In R. Menary (Ed.), The extended mind. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J., Harris, C., Keil, P., & Barnier, A. (2010). The psychology of memory, extended cognition, and socially distributed remembering. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 521–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, K., Garfinkel, S. N., Critchley, H. D., & Seth, A. K. (2013). Multisensory integration across exteroceptive and interoceptive domains modulates self-experience in the rubber-hand illusion. Neuropsychologia, 51(13), 2909–2917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swan, M. (2013). The quantified self: Fundamental disruption in big data science and biological discovery. Big Data, 1(2), 85–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tasali, E., Leproult, R., Ehrmann, D. A., & Van Cauter, E. (2008). Slow-wave sleep and the risk of type 2 diabetes in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(3), 1044–1049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Theiner, G. (2014). Varieties of group cognition. In L. A. Shapiro (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition. New York, New York, USA: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Theiner, G., Allen, C., & Goldstone, R. L. (2010). Recognizing group cognition. Cognitive Systems Research, 11, 378–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toniolo, A., Ouyang, R. W., Dropps, T., Allen, J. A., Johnson, D. P., de Mel, G., Norman, T. J., Oren, N., Srivastava, M. (2014). CISpaces, towards a system for online collaborative argument construction and debate. In International Workshop on Arguing on the Web 2.0, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tribble, E., & Sutton, J. (2011). Cognitive ecology as a framework for Shakespearean studies. Shakespeare Studies, 39, 94–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valins, S. (1966). Cognitive effects of false heart-rate feedback. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4(4), 400–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallar, G., & Ronchi, R. (2009). Somatoparaphrenia: A body delusion. A review of the neuropsychological literature. Experimental Brain Research, 192(3), 533–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Kleek, M., & O’Hara, K. (2014). The future of the social is personal: The potential of the personal data store. In D. Miorandi, V. Maltese, M. Rovatsos, A. Nijholt, & J. Stewart (Eds.), Social collective intelligence: Combining the powers of humans and machines to build a smarter society. Berlin, Germany: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vincent, J., Haddon, L., & Hamill, L. (2005). The influence of mobile phone users on the design of 3G products and services. The Journal of the Communications Network, 4(4), 69–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S., & Luria, A. R. (1994). Tool and symbol in child development. In R. Van Der Veer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webb, B. (1996). A cricket robot. Scientific American, 275(6), 94–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, W., Rabaey, J. M., & Aarts, E. (2005). Ambient intelligence. Berlin, Germany: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (1995). A computer network model of human transactive memory. Social Cognition, 13(3), 319–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiser, M. (1991). The computer for the 21st century. Scientific American, 265(3), 94–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williamson, K., & Sutton, J. (2014). Embodied collaboration in small groups. In C. T. Wolfe (Ed.), Brain theory: Essays in critical neurophilosophy. Basingstoke, England, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. A., & Clark, A. (2009). Situated cognition: Letting nature take its course. In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Won, A. S., Bailenson, J., Lee, J., & Lanier, J. (2015). Homuncular flexibility in virtual reality. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 20(3), 241–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woods, S. P., Iudicello, J. E., Moran, L. M., Carey, C. L., Dawson, M. S., & Grant, I. (2008). HIV-associated prospective memory impairment increases risk of dependence in everyday functioning. Neuropsychology, 22(1), 110–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Paul Smart’s contribution to this work was supported by the U.S. Army Research Laboratory and the U.K. Ministry of Defence and was accomplished as part of the International Technology Alliance (ITA) Project under Agreement Number W911NF-06-3-0001. Some of the ideas contained in the chapter were presented by Richard Heersmink at a seminar at the Center for Human Interactivity of the University of Southern Denmark. Richard would like to thank Sune Steffensen for the invitation and the audience for helpful feedback . Richard would also like to thank John Sutton and Alexander Gillett for helpful discussion on the Internet and cognition. Robert Clowes wishes to acknowledge the support of Portuguese Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia grant/BPD/70440/2010 that made possible his contribution to this paper. All authors would like to thank two anonymous reviewers who provided detailed feedback and suggestions on an earlier version of the chapter.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Smart .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Smart, P., Heersmink, R., Clowes, R.W. (2017). The Cognitive Ecology of the Internet. In: Cowley, S., Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (eds) Cognition Beyond the Brain. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-49115-8_13

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-49115-8_13

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-49114-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-49115-8

  • eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics