Abstract
Nowhere in the entire fossil record of life do we find more dramatically accelerated accumulation of evolutionary novelty than we do in the genus Homo . Quite simply, and by whatever criteria you measure it, our species Homo sapiens is more different from its own precursors of two million years ago than is any other species living in the world today. What might account for this unusually rapid rate of evolution? A major influence was almost certainly material culture , though not in the gene-culture co-evolutionary context envisaged by the evolutionary psychologists. Rather, material culture enhances the ability of hominid populations to disperse at times when conditions are favorable for expansion, while incompletely insulating the resulting enlarged populations from environmental stress when circumstances deteriorate. In other words, by facilitating expansion beyond normal physiological limits in good times, culture makes populations more vulnerable to fragmentation in bad ones. Over the course of the Pleistocene , short-term but large-scale local environmental changes became increasingly frequent over large tracts of the Old World, further amplifying the stress-and-response cycle. Since the fixation probabilities of evolutionary novelties of all kinds (as well as of local extinctions) are promoted by population fragmentation and consequent small effective population sizes, we see in the synergy between environmental effects and material culture a sort of ratchet effect which would have acted to leverage rates of accumulating change. This interaction explains the extraordinarily fast tempo of evolution within the genus Homo by invoking perfectly routine evolutionary processes; and it eliminates any need for special pleading in the hominid case, at least in terms of mechanism. Apparent recent diminution in human brain size may result from greater algorithmic efficiency.
Access this chapter
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Bailey, D. H., & Geary, D. C. (2009). Hominid brain evolution: Climatic, ecological, and social competition models. Human Nature, 20, 67–79.
Beals, K. L., Smith, C. L., & Dodd, S. M. (1984). Brain size, cranial morphology, climate and time machines. Current Anthropology, 25, 301–330.
Collard, M., & Wood, B. (2015). Defining the genus Homo. In W. Henke & I. Tattersall (Eds.), Handbook of paleoanthropology (2nd ed., pp. 1575–1610). Berlin: Springer.
de Heinzelin, J., Clark, J. D., White, T., Hart, W., Renne, P., WoldeGabriel, G., et al. (1999). Environment and behavior of 2.5 million-year-old Bouri hominids. Science, 284, 625–629.
Eldredge, N., & Cracraft, J. L. (1980). Phylogenetic patterns and the evolutionary process. New York: Columbia University Press.
Gabunia, L., Vekua, A., Lordkipanidze, D., Swisher, C. C., Ferring, R., Justus, A., et al. (2000). Earliest Pleistocene hominid cranial remains from Dmanisi, Republic of Georgia: Taxonomy, geological setting and age. Science, 288, 1019–1025.
Gabounia, L., de Lumley, M.-A., Vekua, A., Lordkipanidze, D., & de Lumley, H. (2002). Découverte d’un nouvel hominidé à Dmanissi (Transcaucasie, Géorgie). Comptes Rendus Palevol, 1, 243–253.
Hartwig, W. (Ed.). (2002). The primate fossil record. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hawks, J. (2011). Selection for smaller brains in Holocene human evolution. arXiv:1102.5604v1.
Holloway, R. L., Broadfield, D. C., & Yuan, M. S. (2004). The human fossil record, vol. 3: Hominid endocasts, the paleoneurological evidence. New York: Wiley-Liss.
Kimbel, W. H., Johanson, D. C., & Rak, Y. (1997). Systematic assessment of a maxilla of Homo from Hadar, Ethiopia. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 103, 236–262.
Leakey, M. G., Spoor, F., Brown, F. H., Gathogo, P. N., Leakey, L. N., & McDougall, I. (2001). New hominin genus from eastern Africa shows diverse middle Pliocene lineages. Nature, 410, 433–440.
Lepre, C. J., Roche, H., Kent, D. V., Harmand, S., Quinn, R. L., Brugal, J.-P., et al. (2011). An earlier origin for the Acheulian. Nature, 477, 82–85.
Lumsden, C., & Wilson, E. (1981). Genes, mind and culture: The coevolutionary process. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lumsden, C., & Wilson, E. (1982). Précis of Genes, mind and culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 1–7.
Matsudaira, K., & Ishida, T. (2010). Phylogenetic relationships and divergence dates of the whole mitochondrial genome sequences among three gibbon genera. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, 55, 454–459.
Mercader, J., Barton, H., Gillespie J., Harris, J., Kuhn, S., Tyler, R., et al. (2007). 4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 104, 3043–3048.
Messager, E., Lebreton, V., Marquez, L., Russo-Ermoli, E., Orain, R., Renault-Miskovsky, J., et al. (2011). Palaeoenvironments of early hominins in temperate and Mediterranean Eurasia: New palaeobotanical data from Palaeolithic key-sites and synchronous natural sequences. Quaternary Science Reviews, 30, 1439–1447.
Pickering, R., Dirks, P. G. M., Jinnah, Z., de Ruiter, D., Churchill, S. E., Herries, A. I. R., et al. (2011). Australopithecus sediba at 1.977 Ma and implications for the origins of the genus Homo. Science, 333, 1421–1422.
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Schrenk, F., Bromage, T., Betzler, C., Ring, U., & Juwayeyi, Y. (1993). Oldest Homo and Pliocene biogeography of the Malawi Rift. Nature, 365, 833–836.
Schwartz, J. H., & Tattersall, I. (2005). The human fossil record, vol. 3: Genera Australopithecus, Paranthropus, Orrorin, and overview. New York: Wiley-Liss.
Semaw, S., Renne, P., Harris, J. W. K., Feibel, C. S., Bernor, R. L., Fesseha, N., et al. (1997). 2.5 million-year-old stone tools from Gona. Ethiopia. Nature, 385, 333–336.
Simpson, G. G. (1944). Tempo and mode in evolution. New York: Columbia University Press.
Simpson, G. G. (1953). The major features of evolution. New York: Columbia University Press.
Sponheimer, M., & Lee-Thorp, J. (2007). Hominin paleodiets: The contribution of stable isotopes. In W. Henke & I. Tattersall (Eds.), Handbook of Paleoanthropology (Vol. 1, pp. 554–585). New York: Springer.
Susman, R., Stern, J. T., & Jungers, W. L. (1984). Arboreality and bipedality in the Hadar hominids. Folia Primatologica, 43, 113–156.
Stone, A. C., Battistuzzi, F. U., Kubatko, L. S., Perry, G. H., Jr., Trudeau, E., Lin, H., et al. (2010). More reliable estimates of divergence times in Pan using complete mtDNA sequences and accounting for population structure. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 3277–3288.
Tattersall, I. (2008). An evolutionary framework for the acquisition of symbolic cognition by Homo sapiens. Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews, 3, 99–114.
Tattersall, I. (2012). Masters of the planet: The search for our human origins. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Tattersall, I., & Schwartz, J. H. (2009). Evolution of the genus Homo. Annual Reviews of Earth and Planetary Sciences, 37, 67–92.
Tobias, P. V. (1995). The communication of the dead: Earliest vestiges of the origin of articulate language. Amsterdam: Kroon Lectures, Stichting Nederlands Museum voor Anthropologie en Praehisoire.
Van Andel, T. H., & Davies, W. (Eds.). (2003). Neanderthals and modern humans in the European landscape during the last glaciation. Cambridge, UK: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.
Wood, B., & Collard, M. (1999). The human genus. Science, 284, 65–71.
Wrangham, R. (2011). Chimpanzees, bonobos and the self-domestication hypothesis. American Journal of Primatology, 73, 33–45.
Acknowledgments
My gratitude goes to Clive and Geraldine Finlayson, and to Darren Fa, for inviting me to the splendid Calpe’12 Conference on “The Human Niche: Ecology, Behavior and Culture,” for which the thoughts in this essay were originally gathered. And I equally warmly thank Assaf Marom and Erella Hovers for enabling me to express them in appreciation of our great friend and colleague Yoel Rak. The perceptive comments of two anonymous reviewers improved the manuscript, and Jennifer Steffey kindly prepared the illustrations.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG
About this chapter
Cite this chapter
Tattersall, I. (2017). Why Was Human Evolution So Rapid?. In: Marom, A., Hovers, E. (eds) Human Paleontology and Prehistory. Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-46646-0_1
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-46646-0_1
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-319-46644-6
Online ISBN: 978-3-319-46646-0
eBook Packages: Earth and Environmental ScienceEarth and Environmental Science (R0)