Skip to main content

The Dimensional Divide: Learning from TV and Touchscreens During Early Childhood

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Media Exposure During Infancy and Early Childhood

Abstract

As children’s exposure to touchscreen technology and other digital media increases, so does the need to understand the conditions under which children are able to learn from this technology. The prevalence of screen media in the lives of young children has increased significantly over the last two decades. The use of touchscreen devices among 2–4-year-olds in the USA increased from 39 to 80 % from 2011 to 2013 (Rideout, 2013). Despite frequent engagement with these devices, it is widely recognized that children exhibit a transfer deficit, a term coined to denote children’s consistently poorer learning from television and touchscreens relative to face-to-face interaction (see Barr, Developmental review 30(2):128–154, 2010; Barr, Child Development Perspectives 7(4):205–210, 2013). In this chapter, we focus on understanding the transfer deficit when children engage in imitative learning from touchscreens and television (e.g., Dickerson et al., Developmental Psychobiology 55(7):719–732, 2013, Moser et al., Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 137:137–155, 2015; Zack et al., British Journal of Developmental Psychology 27(Pt 1):13–26, 2009, Zack et al., Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 54(1):20–25, 2013; Zimmermann et al., Child Development, in press). Specifically, we discuss the role of child experience, perceptual and cognitive constraints, transfer distance, and social scaffolding in the transfer deficit. We conclude with lessons for parents and early educators regarding the strategies that may enhance learning across the dimensional divide.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • American Academy of Pediatrics. (1999). Media education. Pediatrics, 104(2), 341–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Academy of Pediatrics. (2011). Media use by children younger than 2 years. Pediatrics, 128, 1040–1045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Academy of Pediatrics. (2013). Children, adolescents, and the media. Pediatrics, 132(5), 958–961.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, D. R., Bryant, J., Wilder, A., Santomero, A., Williams, M. E., & Crawley, A. M. (2000). Researching blue’s clues: Viewing behavior and impact. Media Psychology, 2(2), 179–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, D. R., & Hanson, K. G. (2010). From blooming, buzzing confusion to media literacy: The early development of television viewing. Developmental Review, 30, 239–255. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2010.03.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, D. R., & Pempek, T. A. (2005). Television and very young children. American Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 505–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D. A., & Moses, L. J. (1996). The ontogeny of social information gathering. Child Development, 67(5), 1915–1939.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn?: A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 612–637. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R. (2010). Transfer of learning between 2D and 3D sources during infancy: Informing theory and practice. Developmental Review, 30(2), 128–154. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2010.03.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R. (2013). Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens. Child Development Perspectives, 7(4), 205–210. doi:10.1111/cdep.12041.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., & Brito, N. (2014). From specificity to flexibility: Developmental changes during infancy. In P. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), Wiley-Blackwell handbook on the development of children’s memory (pp. 453–479). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., Danziger, C., Hilliard, M. E., Andolina, C., & Ruskis, J. (2010). Amount, content and context of infant media exposure: A parental questionnaire and diary analysis. International Journal of Early Years Education, 18(2), 107–122.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. Child Development, 70(5), 1067–1081.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., & Hayne, H. (2000). Age-related changes in imitation: Implications for memory development. In C. Rovee-Collier, L. P. Lipsitt, & H. Hayne (Eds.), Progress in infancy research (Vol. 1, pp. 21–67). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., & Hayne, H. (2003). It’s not what you know, it’s who you know: Older siblings facilitate imitation during infancy. International Journal of Early Years Education, 11(1), 7–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., Moser, A., Rusnak, S., Zimmermann, L., Dickerson, K., Lee, H., & Gerhardstein, P. (2016). The impact of memory load and perceptual cues on puzzle learning by 2-year-olds. Developmental Psychobiology, 58, 817–828.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., Muentener, P., & Garcia, A. (2007). Age-related changes in deferred imitation from television by 6- to 18 month olds. Developmental Science, 10, 910–921.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., Muentener, P., Garcia, A., Fujimoto, M., & Chávez, V. (2007). The effect of repetition on imitation from television during infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 49(2), 196–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., & Wyss, N. (2008). Reenactment of televised content by 2 year olds: Toddlers user language learned from television to solve a difficult problem. Infant Behavior and Development, 31, 696–703.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., Wyss, N., & Somanader, M. (2009). The influence of electronic sound effects on learning from televised and live models. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(1), 1–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., Shuck, L., Salerno, K., Atkinson, E., & Linebarger, D. L. (2010). Music interferes with learning from television during infancy. Infant and Child Development, 19(3), 313–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyle, J., & Butler, M. (2016). Supporting children to find their own agency in learning: Commentary on chapter 17. In R. Barr & D. N. Linebarger (Eds.), Media exposure during infancy and early childhood: The effect of content and context on learning and development (pp. 282–290). New York, NY: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-45102-2_18.

  • Brito, N., Barr, R., McIntyre, P., & Simcock, G. (2012). Long-term transfer of learning from books and video during toddlerhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(1), 108–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burgaleta, M., Johnson, W., Waber, D. P., Colom, R., & Karama, S. (2014). Cognitive ability changes and dynamics of cortical thickness development in healthy children and adolescents. NeuroImage, 84, 810–819.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51(13), 1484–1525.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In Y. Munakata & M. H. Johnson (Eds.), Processes of change in brain and cognitive development. Attention and performance XXI (Vol. 21, pp. 249–274). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037–2078.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S., Simcock, G., & Marzolf, D. P. (2004). Transfer by very young children in the symbolic retrieval task. Child Development, 75(6), 1708–1718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dickerson, K., Gerhardstein, P., Zack, E., & Barr, R. (2013). Age-related changes in learning across early childhood: A new imitation task. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(7), 719–732. doi:10.1002/dev.21068.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, S. (1993). Class similarity and viewpoint invariance in the recognition of 3D objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerson, S. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2013). The goal trumps the means: Highlighting goals is more beneficial than highlighting means in means‐end training. Infancy, 18(2), 289–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M. H., Waterfall, H. R., Lotem, A., Halpern, J. Y., Schwade, J. A., Onnis, L., et al. (2010). General cognitive principles for learning structure in time and space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 249–258. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.02.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodrich, S. A., Pempek, T. A., & Calvert, S. L. (2009). Formal production features of infant and toddler DVDs. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 163(12), 1151–1156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayne, H. (2004). Infant memory development: Implications for childhood amnesia. Developmental Review, 24(1), 33–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayne, H., & Herbert, J. (2004). Verbal cues facilitate memory retrieval during infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89(2), 127–139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayne, H., Herbert, J., & Simcock, G. (2003). Imitation from television by 24- and 30 month olds. Developmental Science, 6(3), 254–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herbert, J., & Hayne, H. (2000). Memory retrieval by 18–30 month olds: Age-related changes in representational flexibility. Developmental Psychology, 36(4), 473–484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hickling, A. K., & Wellman, H. M. (2001). The emergence of children’s causal explanations and theories: Evidence from everyday conversation. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 668.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., Zosh, J. M., Golinkoff, R. M., Gray, J. H., Robb, M. B., & Kaufman, J. (2015). Putting education in “educational” apps lessons from the science of learning. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 16(1), 3–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, T. C., & Charman, C. (2005). Graduations of emulation learning in infant’s imitation of actions on objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 276–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krcmar, M. (2010). Can social meaningfulness and repeat exposure help infants and toddlers overcome the video deficit? Media Psychology, 13(1), 31–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krcmar, M. (2014). Can infants and toddlers learn words from repeat exposure to an infant directed DVD? Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 58(2), 196–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lauricella, A., Barr, R., & Calvert, S. L. (2009). Emerging computer skills: Influences of young children’s executive functioning abilities and parental scaffolding techniques. Journal of Children and Media, 3(3), 217–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lauricella, A., Barr, R., & Calvert, S. L. (2014). Parent–child interactions during traditional and computer storybook reading for children’s comprehension: Implications for electronic storybook design. International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction, 2(1), 17–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lauricella, A. R., Blackwell, C. K., & Wartella, E. (2016). The “New” technology environment: The role of content and context on learning and development from mobile media. In R. Barr & D. N. Linebarger (Eds.), Media exposure during infancy and early childhood: The effects of content and context on learning and development. New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-45102-2_1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, S. C., Ratliff, K. R., Huttenlocher, J., & Cannon, J. (2012). Early puzzle play: A predictor of preschoolers’ spatial transformation skill. Developmental Psychology, 48(2), 530–542. doi:10.1037/a0025913.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liebeskind, K. G., & Bryant, A. (2016). Bridging the dimensional divide in the real world: Commentary on chapter 3. In R. Barr & D. N. Linebarger (Eds.), Media exposure during infancy and early childhood: The effects of content and context on learning and development. New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-45102-2_4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahajan, N., & Woodward, A. (2009). Infants imitate human agents but not inanimate objects. Infancy, 14(6), 667–679.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McClure, E., & Barr, R. (2016). Building family relationships from a distance: Supporting connections with babies and toddlers using video and video chat. In R. Barr & D. N. Linebarger (Eds.), Media exposure during infancy and early childhood: The effects of content and context on learning and development. New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-45102-2_15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1990). Towards a developmental cognitive science: The implications of cross-modal matching and imitation for the development of representation and memory in infancy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608, 1–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). ‘Like me’: a foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science, 10(1), 126–134.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 954.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S. E., & Marcovitch, S. (2011). Toddlers benefit from labeling on an executive function search task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 580–592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moser, A., Zimmermann, L., Dickerson, K., Grenell, A., Barr, R., & Gerhardstein, P. (2015). They can interact, but can they learn? Toddlers’ transfer learning from touchscreens and television. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 137, 137–155. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.04.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., & Blank, C. (2011). Imitation in young children: When who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 1050.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., Simcock, G., & Jenkins, L. (2008). The effect of social engagement on 24 month olds’ imitation from live and televised models. Developmental Science, 11(5), 722–731. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00722.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Putting the social into social learning: Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Qian, N. (1997). Binocular disparity and the perception of depth. Neuron, 18(3), 359–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radesky, J. S., Schumacher, J., & Zuckerman, B. (2015). Mobile and interactive media use by young children: The good, the bad, and the unknown. Pediatrics, 135(1), 1–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rideout, V. (2013). Zero to eight: Children’s media use in America 2013. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robb, M. (2016). What’s in a look? How young children learn from screen media and implications for early educators: Commentary on chapter 5. In R. Barr & D. N. Linebarger (Eds.), Media exposure during infancy and early childhood: The effects of content and context on learning and development. New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-45102-2_6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roseberry, S., Hirsh‐Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014). Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development, 85, 956–970.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, K. L., & Anderson, D. R. (2002). Television and reality: Toddlers’ use of visual information from video to guide behavior. Media Psychology, 4(1), 51–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seehagen, S., & Herbert, J. (2010). The role of demonstrator familiarity and language cues on infant imitation from television. Infant Behavior and Development, 33(2), 168–175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sella, I., Reiner, M., & Pratt, H. (2014). Natural stimuli from three coherent modalities enhance behavioral responses and electrophysiological cortical activity in humans. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 93(1), 45–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simcock, G., & DeLoache, J. (2006). Get the picture? The effects of iconicity on toddlers’ reenactment from picture books. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1352–1357. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simcock, G., & Dooley, M. (2007). Generalization of learning from picture books to novel test conditions by 18- and 24 month old children. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1568.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simcock, G., Garrity, K., & Barr, R. (2011). The effect of narrative cues on infants’ imitation from television and picture books. Child Development, 82(5), 1607–1619. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01636.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Simcock, G., & Hayne, H. (2002). Breaking the barrier? Children fail to translate their preverbal memories into language. Psychological Science, 13(3), 225–231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strouse, G. A., & Troseth, G. L. (2014). Supporting toddlers’ transfer of word learning from video. Cognitive Development, 30, 47–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Subiaul, F., Anderson, S., Brandt, J., & Elkins, J. (2012). Multiple imitation mechanisms in children. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T. (2003). Early representational insight: twenty-four month olds can use a photo to find an object in the world. Child Development, 74, 896–904.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., Simcock, G., & Nielsen, M. (2007). Visual self-recognition in mirrors and live videos: Evidence for a developmental asynchrony. Cognitive Development, 22(2), 185–196. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.09.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Push or pull: Imitation vs. emulation in great apes and human children. Ethology, 112(12), 1159–1169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L., & Woodworth, R. S. (1901). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. II. The estimation of magnitudes. Psychological Review, 8(4), 384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Troseth, G. L. (2003a). Getting a clear picture: Young children’s understanding of a televised image. Developmental Science, 6(3), 247–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Troseth, G. L. (2003b). TV guide: two-year-old children learn to use video as a source of information. Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 140–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Troseth, G. L. (2010). Is it life or is it Memorex? Video as a representation of reality. Developmental Review, 30(2), 155–175. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2010.03.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Troseth, G. L., & DeLoache, J. S. (1998). The medium can obscure the message: Young children’s understanding of video. Child Development, 69(4), 950–965.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Troseth, G. L., Saylor, M. M., & Archer, A. H. (2006). Young children’s use of video as a source of socially relevant information. Child Development, 77, 786–799.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zack, E., Barr, R., Gerhardstein, P., Dickerson, K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2009). Infant imitation from television using novel touch screen technology. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(Pt 1), 13–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zack, E., Gerhardstein, P., Meltzoff, A. N., & Barr, R. (2013). 15 Month olds’ transfer of learning between touch screen and real-world displays: Language cues and cognitive loads. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 54(1), 20–25. doi:10.1111/sjop.12001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmermann, L., Moser, A., Grenell, A., Dickerson, K., Yao, Q., Gerhardstein, P., & Barr, R. (2015). Do semantic contextual cues facilitate transfer learning from video in toddlers? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 561. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00561

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmermann, L., Moser, A., Gerhardstein, P., & Barr, R. (in press). The ghost in the touchscreen: Social scaffolds promote learning by toddlers. Child Development.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zosh, J. M., Lytle, S. R., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2016). Putting the education back in educational apps: How content and context interact to promote learning. In R. Barr & D. N. Linebarger (Eds.), Media exposure during infancy and early childhood: The effects of content and context on learning and development. New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-45102-2_17.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Support for this chapter was provided by NSF BCS grant 1023772 to Peter Gerhardstein and Rachel Barr.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter Gerhardstein .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hipp, D., Gerhardstein, P., Zimmermann, L., Moser, A., Taylor, G., Barr, R. (2017). The Dimensional Divide: Learning from TV and Touchscreens During Early Childhood. In: Barr, R., Linebarger, D. (eds) Media Exposure During Infancy and Early Childhood. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-45102-2_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics