Abstract
General fluid intelligence (Gf) is the ability used in inductive and deductive reasoning, particularly with novel material. It can be contrasted with general crystallized ability (Gc) which reflects schooling and acculturated learning, and the two abilities have different developmental trajectories, with Gf peaking earlier in the lifespan. Gustafsson has made key contributions to our understanding of Gf. He (Gustafsson 1984) introduced hierarchical confirmatory factor analytic models to reconcile Thurstonian (non-hierarchical) and Spearman and Cattell-Horn (hierarchical) models of intelligence and in so doing identified Gf as a second-order factor which perfectly correlated with the third-order factor, general ability (g). This has important implications for understanding the nature of general cognitive skill. Subsequent research showed that Gf can be identified separately from g through variation in culture-related opportunities to learn (Valentin Kvist and Gustafsson 2008). Gf has served both as a predictor (Gustafsson and Balke 1993) and outcome (Cliffordson and Gustafsson 2008) in the developmental, cognitive training, cognitive aging, international comparative assessment, genetics, neuropsychopharmacological, human capital theory, and behavioral economics literatures. Understanding the nature of fluid intelligence and how to improve it has become a topic of renewed and general interest for optimizing human performance in school and in the workplace.
Access this chapter
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Ackerman, P. L. (1996). A theory of adult intellectual development: Process personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence, 22, 229–259.
Ackerman, P. L. (1999). Traits and knowledge as determinants of learning and individual differences: Putting it all together. In P. L. Ackerman, P. C. Kyllonen, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), Learning and individual differences: Process, trait, and content determinants (pp. 437–460). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 30–60.
Arendasy, M., & Sommer, M. (2005). Using psychometric technology in educational assessment: The case of a schema-based isomorphic approach to the automatic generation of quantitative reasoning items. Learning and Individual Differences, 17(4), 366–383.
Arendasy, M., & Sommer, M. (2012). Using automatic item generation to meet the increasing item demands of high-stakes assessment. Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 112–117.
Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2015). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(2), 366–377.
Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839. doi:10.1038/nrn1201
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.
Battleday, R. M., & Brem, A. K. (2015). Modafinil for cognitive neuroenhancement in healthy non-sleep-deprived subjects: A systematic review. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 25, 1865–1881.
Becker, M., Lüdtke, O., Trautwein, U., Köller, O., & Baumert, J. (2012). The differential effects of school tracking on psychometric intelligence: Do academic-track schools make students smarter? Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 682–699. doi:10.1037/a0027608
Birney, D. P., & Bowman, D. B. (2009). An experimental-differential investigation of cognitive complexity. Psychology Science Quarterly, 51(4), 449–469.
Borghans, L., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. J., & ter Weel, B. (2008, February). The economics and psychology of personality traits (NBER Working Paper No. 13810). Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.
Borghans, L., Golsteyn, B. H. H., Heckman, J. J., & Humphries, J. E. (2015). What do grades and achievement tests measure? Unpublished manuscript, Human Capital and Economic Working Group, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL. Retrieved from https://hceconomics.uchicago.edu/sites/default/files/3_BGHH-Grades-Achievement-Measure-Paper_2015-12-30a_sjs.pdf
Boring, E. G. (1923). Intelligence as the tests test it. New Republic, 36, 35–37.
Bridgeman, B., Strang, H. R., & Buttram, J. (1974). “Game” versus “test” instructions for the WISC. Journal of Educational Measurement, 11, 285–287.
Brinch, C. N., & Galloway, T. A. (2011). Schooling in adolescence raises IQ scores. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(2), 425–430. doi:10.1073/pnas.1106077109
Burgess, G. C., Gray, J. R., Conway, A. R. A., & Braver, T. (2011). Neural mechanisms of interference control underlie the relationship between fluid intelligence and working memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 674–692.
Buros, O. K. (Ed.). (1965). The sixth mental measurements yearbook. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. ISBN 0-910674-06-X.
Cahan, S., & Cohen, N. (1989). Age versus schooling effects on intelligence development. Child Development, 60(5), 1239–1249.
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97, 404–431.
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Carroll, J. B. (2003). The higher-stratum structure of cognitive abilities: Current evidence supports g and about ten broad factors. In H. Nyborg (Ed.), The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (pp. 5–21). Boston, MA: Pergamon.
Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54(1), 1–22.
Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth, and action. New York: North-Holland.
Cattell, R. B., & Horn, J. L. (1978). A check on the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence with description of new subtest designs. Journal of Educational Measurement, 15, 139–164.
Ceci, S. (1991). How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence. Developmental Psychology, 27(5), 703–722.
Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (1997). Schooling, intelligence, and income. American Psychologist, 52, 1051–1058.
Chuderski, A. (2015). The broad factor of working memory is virtually isomorphic to fluid intelligence tested under time pressure. Personality and Individual Differences, 85, 98–104.
Cliffordson, C. (2010). Methodological issues in investigations of the relative effects of schooling and age on school performance: The between-grade regression discontinuity design applied to Swedish TIMSS 1995 data. Educational Research and Evaluation: An International Journal on Theory and Practice, 16(1), 39–52.
Cliffordson, C., & Gustafsson, J.-E. (2008). Effects of age and schooling on intellectual performance: Estimates obtained from analysis of continuous variation in age and length of schooling. Intelligence, 36, 143–152.
Conway, A. R. A., Jarrold, C., Kane, M. J., Miyake, A., & Towse, J. N. (Eds.). (2007). Variations in working memory. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Corno, L., Cronbach, L. J., Kupermintz, H., Lohman, D. F., Mandinach, E. B., Porteus, A. W., et al. (2002). Remaking the concept of aptitude: Extending the legacy of Richard E. Snow. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cronbach, L. J. (1971). Test validation. In R. L. Thorndike (Ed.), Educational measurement (2nd ed., pp. 443–507). Washington, DC: American Council on Education.
Dance, A. (2016). A dose of intelligence. Nature, 536, S2–S3.
Danziger, K. (2009). The holy grail of universality. In T. Teo, P. Stenner, A. Rutherford, E. Park, & C. Baerveldt (Eds.), Varieties of philosophical and practical concerns (pp. 2–11). Concord, Ontario, Canada: Captus University Publications.
Deary, I. J., Starr, J. M., & MacLennan, W. J. (1998). Fluid intelligence, memory and blood pressure in cognitive aging. Personality and Individual Differences, 25(4), 605–619.
Diehl, K. A. (2002). Algorithmic item generation and problem solving strategies in matrix completion problems. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas.
Duckworth, A. L., Quinn, P. D., Lynam, D. R., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2011). Role of test motivation in intelligence testing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 7716–7720.
Embretson, S. E. (2002). Generating abstract reasoning items with cognitive theory. In S. Irvine & P. Kyllonen (Eds.), Generating items for cognitive tests: Theory and practice. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Finn, A. S., Kraft, M., West, M. R., Leonard, J. A., Bish, C., Martin, R. E., … Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2014). Cognitive skills, student student achievement tests and schools. Psychological Science, 25, 736–744.
Fiske, D. W., & Butler, J. M. (1963). The experimental conditions for measuring individual differences. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 26, 249–266.
Flynn, J. R. (2007). What is intelligence? Beyond the Flynn effect. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Flynn, J. R., & Weiss, L. G. (2007). American IQ gains from 1932 to 2002: The WISC subtests and educational progress. International Journal of Testing, 7, 209–224.
Freund, P. A., & Holling, H. (2011). How to get really smart: Modeling retest and training effects in ability testing using computer-generated figural matrix items. Intelligence, 39, 233–243.
Geggel, L. (2015). ‘Smart drug’ modafinil actually works, study shows. Live Science. Retrieved April 1, 2016 from http://www.livescience.com/51919-modafil-improves-attention-learning.html
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Gustafsson, J.-E. (1984). A unifying model for the structure of intellectual abilities. Intelligence, 8, 179–203.
Gustafsson, J.-E. (1998). Hierarchical models of individual differences in cooperative abilities. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 4, pp. 35–71).
Gustafsson, J.-E. (1999). Measuring and understanding G: Experimental and correlational approaches. In P. L. Ackerman, P. C. Kyllonen, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), Learning and individual differences: Process, trait, and content determinants (pp. 275–289). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.
Gustafsson, J.-E. (2001). Schooling and intelligence: Effects of track of study on level and profile of cognitive abilities. International Education Journal, 2(4), 166–186.
Gustafsson, J.-E. (2002). Measurement from a hierarchical point of view. In H. L. Braun, D. G. Jackson, & D. E. Wiley (Eds.), The role of constructs in psychological and educational measurement (pp. 73–95). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gustafsson, J.-E., & Balke, G. (1993). General and specific abilities as predictors of school achievement. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 28(4), 407–434.
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803–865.
Harden, K. P., Turkheimer, E., & Loehlin, J. C. (2007). Genotype by environment interaction in adolescents’ cognitive aptitude. Behavior Genetics, 37(2), 273–283.
Harkins, S. G. (2006). Mere effort as the mediator of the evaluation-performance relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 436–455. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.436
Heckman, J. J., & Kautz, T. (2014). Achievement tests and the role of character in American life. In J. J. Heckman, J. E. Humphries, & T. Kautz (Eds.), The myth of achievement tests: The GED and the role of character in American life (pp. 3–56). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve: Intelligence and class structure in American life. New York, NY: The Free Press (Simon & Shuster).
Horn, J. L. (1989). Models for intelligence. In R. Linn (Ed.), Intelligence: Measurement, theory, and public policy (pp. 29–73). Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Horn, J. L. (2008). Spearman, g, expertise, and the nature of human cognitive capability. In P. C. Kyllonen, R. D. Roberts, & L. Stankov (Eds.), Extending intelligence: Enhancement and new constructs (pp. 185–230). New York: LEA.
Horn, J. L., & Noll, J. (1997). Human cognitive capabilities: Gf-Gc theory. In D. P. Flanagan, J. L. Genshaft, & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests and issues (pp. 53–91). New York, NY: Guilford.
Hornke, L. F., & Habon, M. W. (1986). Rule-based item bank construction and evaluation within the linear logistic framework. Applied Psychological Measurement, 10, 369–380.
Hunt, E. (2011). Human intelligence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Irvine, S. H. (2002). The foundations of item generation for mass testing. In S. H. Irvine & P. C. Kyllonen (Eds.), Item generation for test development (pp. 3–34). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Jacobs, P. I., & Vandeventer, M. (1972). Evaluating the teaching of intelligence. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 32(2), 235–238.
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(19), 6829–6833. doi:10.1073/pnas.0801268105
Jensen, E. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability (Human evolution, behavior, and intelligence). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., Wilhelm, O., Payne, T. W., & Engle, R. W. (2004). The generality of working-memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 189–217.
Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (2004). Aging, adult development, and work motivation. The Academy of Management Review, 29(3), 440–458.
Kaufman, J. C. (2012). Self estimates of general, crystallized, and fluid intelligences in an ethnically diverse population. Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 118–122.
Kyllonen, P. C. (1995). CAM: A theoretical framework for cognitive abilities measurement. In D. K. Detterman (Ed.), Current topics in human intelligence (Vol. 4, pp. 307–359). New York, NY: Springer.
Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelligence, 14(4), 389–433.
Lindqvist, E., & Vestman, R. (2011). The labor market returns to cognitive and noncognitive ability: Evidence from the Swedish enlistment. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 3, 101–128.
Liu, O. L., Bridgeman, B., & Adler, R. (2012). Learning outcomes assessment in higher education: Motivation matters. Educational Researcher, 41, 352–362.
McArdle, J. J., Ferrer-Caja, E., Hamagami, F., & Woodcock, R. W. (2002). Comparative longitudinal structural analyses of the growth and decline of multiple intellectual abilities over the life span. Developmental Psychology, 38(1), 115–142.
McGrew, K. S. (1997). Analysis of the major intelligence batteries according to a proposed comprehensive Gf-Gc framework. In D. P. Flanagan, J. L. Genshaft, & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (pp. 151–179). New York, NY: Guilford.
McGrew, K. S. (2005). The Cattell-Horn-Carroll theory of cognitive abilities: Past, present, and future. In D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (2nd ed., pp. 136–182). New York, NY: Guilford.
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2012). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270–291.
Mellers, B., Stone, E., Murray, T., Minster, A., Rohrbaugh, N., Bishop, M., … Tetlock, P. (2015). Identifying and cultivating superforecasters as a method of improving probabilistic predictions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(3), 267–281.
Melnick, M. D., Harrison, B. R., Park, S., Bennetto, L., & Tadin, D. (2013). A strong interactive link between sensory discriminations and intelligence. Current Biology, 23(11), 1013–1017.
Meo, M., Roberts, M. J., & Marucci, F. S. (2007). Element salience as a predictor of item difficulty for Raven’s progressive matrices. Intelligence, 35(4), 359–368.
Mulholland, T. M., Pellegrino, J. W., & Glaser, R. (1980). Components of geometric analogy solution. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 252–284.
Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., et al. (2012). Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67(2), 130–159.
Oberauer, K., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., & Suess, H.-M. (2005). Working memory and intelligence—Their correlation and relation: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Pscyhological Bulletin, 131, 61–65.
Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., et al. (2010). Putting brain training to the test. Nature, 465, 775–778.
Pind, J., Gunnarsdóttir, E. K., Jóhannesson, H. S. (2003). Raven’s standard progressive matrices: New school age norms and a study of the test’s validity. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 375–386.
Postlethwaite, B. E. (2011). Fluid ability, crystallized ability, and performance across multiple domains: A meta-analysis. Doctoral dissertation. Retrieved from http://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/1255/
Preckel, F. (2003). Diagnostik intellektueller Hochbegabung. Testentwicklung zur Erfassung der fluiden Intelligenz [Assessment of intellectual giftedness. Development of a fluid intelligence test]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
Preckel, F., & Thiemann, H. (2003). Online- versus paper-pencil-version of a high potential intelligence test. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 62, 131–138.
Primi, R. (2001). Complexity of geometric inductive reasoning tasks: Contribution to the understanding of fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 20, 41–70.
Rindermann, H. (2007). The g-factor of international cognitive ability comparisons: The homogeneity of results in PISA, TIMSS, PIRLS and IQ-tests across nations. European Journal of Personality, 21, 667–706.
Sackett, P. R. (2012). Cognitive tests, constructs, and content validity: A commentary on Schmidt (2012). International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 20(1)
Schaie, K. W. (2012). Developmental influences on adult intelligence: Seattle Longitudinal Study (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Schalke, D., Brunner, M., Geiser, C., Preckel, F., Keller, U., Spendler, M., et al. (2013). Stability and change in intelligence from age 12 to age 52: Results for the Luxembourg MAGRIP study. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1529–1543.
Schank, R. C. (1984). How much intelligence is there in artificial intelligence? Intelligence, 4(1), 1–14.
Schmid, J., & Leiman, J. M. (1957). The development of hierarchical factor solutions. Psychometrika, 22(1), 53–61.
Schmidt, F. L., & Crano, W. D. (1974). A test of the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence in middle- and low-socioeconomic-status children: A cross-lagged panel analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66(2), 255–261.
Schrank, F. A. (2006). Specification of the cognitive processes involved in performance on the Woodcock-Johnson III (Assessment Service Bulletin No. 7). Itasca, IL: Riverside.
Sechrest, L. (2005). Validity of measures is no simple matter. Health Services Research, 40(5), 1584–1604.
Sells, S. B., Dailey, J. T., & Pickrel, E. W. (Eds.). (1984). Selection of air traffic controllers. Report no. FAA-AM-84-2. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Transportation, Federal Aviation Administration.
Snow, R. E., Kyllonen, P. C., & Marshalek, B. (1984). The topography of ability and learning correlations. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 2., pp. 47–103). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Spearman, C. (1904). “General intelligence,” objectively determined and measured. The American Journal of Psychology, 15(2), 201–292.
Spearman, C. (1924). The nature of “intelligence” and the principles of cognition. Journal of Philosophy, 21(11), 294–301.
Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. London, England: Macmillan and Co.
Stankov, L., & Schweizer, K. (2007). Raven’s progressive matrices, manipulations of complexity and measures of accuracy, speed and confidence. Psychology Science, 49, 326–342.
Stelzl, I., Merz, F., Ehlers, T., & Remer, H. (1995). The effect of schooling on the development of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A quasi-experimental study. Intelligence, 21, 279–296. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(95)90018-7
Terman, L. M. (1924). The mental test as a psychological method. Psychological Review, 31, 93–117.
Thorsen, C., Gustafsson, J.-E., & Cliffordson, C. (2014). The influence of fluid and crystallized intelligence on the development of knowledge and skills. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(4), 556–570. doi:10.1111/bjep.12041
Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Psychometric monographs, 1, ix–121.
Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Salthouse, T. A. (2011). Individual differences in cognitive aging. In T. Chamorro-Premuzic, S. von Stumm, & A. Furnham (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of individual differences (pp. 242–267). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Undheim, J. O. (1981). On Intelligence III: Examining developmental implications of Catail’s broad ability theory and of an alternative neo-Spearman model. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 22(1), 243–249.
Undheim, J. O., & Gustafsson, J.-E. (1987). The hierarchical organization of cognitive abilities: Restoring general intelligence through the use of linear structural relations (LISREL). Multivariate Behavioral Research 22, 149–171.
Unsworth, N., Fukuda, K., Awh, E., & Vogel, E. K. (2014). Working memory and fluid intelligence: Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory. Cognitive Psychology, 71, 1–26.
Valentin Kvist, A. V., & Gustafsson, J.-E. (2008). The relation between fluid intelligence and the general factor as a function of cultural background: A test of Cattell’s investment theory. Intelligence, 36, 422–436.
Wai, J., & Putallaz, M. (2011). The Flynn effect puzzle: A 30-year examination from the right tail of the ability distribution provides some missing pieces. Intelligence, 39(6), 443–455.
Wilhelm, O. (2000). Psychologie des schlussfolgernden Denkens: Differentialpsychologische Prfung von Strukturuberlegeungen [Psychology of reasoning: Testing structural theories]. Hamburg, Germany: Dr. Kovac.
Wilhelm, O. (2006). Measuring reasoning ability. In O. Wilhelm & R. W. Engle (Eds.), Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (pp. 373–392). Los Angeles: Sage. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781452233529.n21
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III. Itasca, IL: Riverside.
Zigler, E., & Trickett, P. K. (1978). IQ, social competence, and the evaluation of early childhood intervention programs. American Psychologist, 33, 789–798.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG
About this chapter
Cite this chapter
Kyllonen, P., Kell, H. (2017). What Is Fluid Intelligence? Can It Be Improved?. In: Rosén, M., Yang Hansen, K., Wolff, U. (eds) Cognitive Abilities and Educational Outcomes. Methodology of Educational Measurement and Assessment. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-43473-5_2
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-43473-5_2
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-319-43472-8
Online ISBN: 978-3-319-43473-5
eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)