Skip to main content

Individual Differences in the Attraction to Sad Music

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

In this chapter Garrido reviews several of the personality traits that have been demonstrated to be associated with a liking for sad music in recent research. Traits such as absorption, empathy, openness to experience and introversion are discussed in the light of key theories such as Schubert’s Dissociation Theory of Emotion in Aesthetic Contexts (DTEAC). Garrido also presents evidence suggesting that people with tendencies to rumination and depression may be attracted to sad music despite the fact that listening to it does not always have positive effects on their mood.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    See Chap. 5 for more details.

References

  • Altenmüller, E., Schurmann, K., Lim, V. K., & Parlitz, D. (2002). Hits to the left, flops to the right: Different emotions during listening to music are reflected in cortical laterisation patterns. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2242–2256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV) (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnhofer, T., Kuehn, E.-M., de Jong-Meyer, R., & Williams, J. M. G. (2006). Beliefs about the benefits of rumination in depressed men and women with and without a history of assault. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 35, 317–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Knickmeyer, R. C., & Belmonte, M. K. (2005). Sex differences in the brain: Implications for explaining autism. Science, 310, 819–823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berdan, L. E., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2008). Temperament and externalizing behvior: Social preference and perceived acceptance as protective factors. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 957–968.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, E. M., & Putnam, F. W. (1986). Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174(12), 727–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 11818–11823.

    Google Scholar 

  • Briere, J., Weathers, F. W., & Runtz, M. (2005). Is dissociation a multidimensional construct? Data from the multiscale dissociation inventory. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18(3), 221–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, L. D. (2006). Normative dissociation. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 29, 45–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, P. S., Connell, C., & Beegle, A. (2007). Adolescents’ expressed meanings of music in and out of school. Journal of Research in Music Education, 55(3), 220–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2007). Personality and music: Can traits explain how people use music in everyday life? British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), 175–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charland, L. C. (2005). The heat of emotion: Valence and the demarcation problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 12(8–10), 82–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colombetti, G. (2005). Appraising valence. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 12(8), 103–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooke, D. (1963). Language of music. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cross, I. (2007). Music, science and culture. In I. Roth (Ed.), Imaginative minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1991). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwall, S. (1997). Empathy, sympathy, care. Philosophical Studies, 89, 261–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dollinger, S. J. (1993). Research note: Personality and music preference: Extraversion and excitement seeking or openness to experience? Psychology of Music, 21(1), 73–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisen, M. L., & Lynn, S. J. (2001). Dissociation, memory and suggestibility in adults and children. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, S49–S73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, P., & Schubert, E. (2008). Relationships between expressed and felt emotions in music. Musicae Scientiae, 12, 75–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabrielsson, A. (2001). Emotions in strong experiences with music. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Theory and research (pp. 431–449). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrido, S., & Schubert, E. (2011). Individual differences in the enjoyment of negative emotion in music: A literature review and experiment. Music Perception, 28(3), 279–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garrido, S., & Schubert, E. (2013). Adaptive and maladaptive attraction to negative emotion in music. Musicae Scientiae, 17(2), 145–164. doi:10.1177/1029864913478305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geen, R. G. (1984). Preferred stimulation levels in introverts and extroverts: Effects on arousal and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(6), 1303–1312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glisky, M. L., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1993). Hypnotizability and facets of openness. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 41, 112–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48, 26–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grewe, O., Nagel, F., Kopiez, R., & Altenmüller, E. (2007). Listening to music as a re-creative process: Physiological, psychological, and psychoacoustical correlates of chills and strong emotions. Music Perception, 24, 297–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, A. R. (2001). Cerebral substrates of musical imagery. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 930(1), 179–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hampson, S. E., & Coleman, A. M. (Eds.). (1995). Individual differences and personality. London and New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herbert, R. (2011). Everyday music listening: Absorption, dissociation, and trancing. Surrey, UK: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herbert, R. (2012). Musical and non-musical involvement in daily life: The case of absorption. Musicae Scientiae, 16(1), 41–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogue, J. D., Crimmins, A. M., & Kahn, J. H. (2015). “So sad and slow, so why can’t I turn off the radio”: The effects of gender, depression, and absorption on liking music that induces sadness and music that induces happiness. Psychology of Music. doi:10.1177/0305735615594489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huron, D. (2011). Why is sad music pleasurable? A possible role for prolactin. Musicae Scientiae, 15(2), 146–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (1987). Young childrens spontaneous personification as analogy. Child Development, 58(4), 1013–1020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juslin, P. N. (2013). From everyday emotions to aesthetic emotions: Towards a unified theory of musical emotions. Physics of Life Reviews, 10, 235–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawakami, A., & Katahira, K. (2015). Influence of trait empahty on the emotion evoked by sad music and on the preference for it. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1541. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kivy, P. (1989). Sound sentiment. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kreutz, G., Ott, U., Teichmann, D., Osawa, P., & Vaitl, D. (2008a). Using music to induce emotion: Influences of musical preference and absorption. Psychology of Music, 36, 101–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreutz, G., Schubert, E., & Mitchell, L. A. (2008b). Cognitive styles of music listening. Music Perception, 26(1), 57–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ladinig, O., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2012). Liking unfamiliar music: Effects of felt emotion and individual differences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 6(2), 146–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leavitt, F. (2001). MMPI profile characteristics of women with varying levels of normal dissociation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(12), 1469–1477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, J. (2009). How we decide. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, J. (1996a). Music and negative emotions. In J. Robinson (Ed.), Music and meaning. New York: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Litle, P., & Zuckerman, M. (1986). Sensation seeking and music preferences. Personality and Individual Differences, 7(4), 575–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martindale, C. (1988). Aesthetics, psychobiology, and cognition. In F. H. Harley & R. W. Neperud (Eds.), The foundations of aesthetics, art and art education (pp. 7–42). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCown, W., Keiser, R., Shea, M., & Williamson, D. (1997). The role of personality and gender in preference for exaggerated bass in music. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(4), 543–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & Costa Jr., P. T. (1985). Openness to experience. Perspectives in Personality, 1, 145–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merckelbah, H., Horselenberg, R., & Muris, P. (2001). The Creative Experiences Questionnaire (CEQ): A brief self-report measure of fantasy proneness. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 987–995.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molnar-Szakacs, I., & Overy, K. (2006). Music and mirror neurons: From motion to ‘e’motion. SCAN, 1, 235–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, C. D., Fox, J., & Pettifer, S. (2007). Absorption, dissociation, locus of control and presence in virtual reality. Computers in Human Behavior, 23(3), 1347–1354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murry, J. P., & Dacin, P. A. (1996). Cognitive moderators of negative-emotion effects: Implications for understanding media context. Journal of Consumer Research, 22, 439–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nettle, D. (2007). A module for metaphor? The site of imagination in the architecture of the mind. In I. Roth (Ed.), Imaginative minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effect on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 569–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & O’Neill, S. A. (2000). The importance of music to adolescents. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 255–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oliver, M. B. (1993). Exploring the paradox of the enjoyment of sad films. Human Communication Research, 19(3), 315–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2001). Metacognitive beliefs about rumination in recurrent major depression. Cognitive and Behavioural Practice, 8, 160–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, R. A., & Kern, R. M. (1996). Changing highbrow taste: From snob to omnivore. American Sociological Review, 61(5), 900–907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pica, M., & Beere, D. (1995). Dissociation during positive situations. Dissociation, VIII(4), 241–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ray, W. J., June, K., Turaj, K., & Lundy, R. (1992). Dissociative experiences in a college age population: A factor analytic study of two dissociation scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 13(4), 417–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rentfrow, P. J., Goldberg, L. R., & Levitin, D. J. (2011). The structure of musical preferences: A five-factor model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(6), 1139–1157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riley, K. C. (1988). Measurement of dissociation. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176(7), 449–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, T. O., Weaver, J. B., & Zillmann, D. (1996). Exploring the relation between personality and the appreciation of rock music. Psychology Report, 78(1), 259–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, C. A. (1996). History, phenomenology, and epidemiology of dissociation. In L. K. Michelson & W. J. Ray (Eds.), Handbook of dissociation: Theoretical, empirical and clinical perspectives. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, I. (2007). Introduction: Imaginative minds: Concepts, controversies and themes. In I. Roth (Ed.), Imaginative minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sachs, M. E., Damasio, A., & Habibi, A. (2015). The pleasures of sad music: A systematic review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 404. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saver, J. L., & Rabin, J. (1997). The neural substrates of religious experiences. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosience, 9, 498–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, E. (1996). Enjoyment of negative emotions in music: An associative network explanation. Psychology of Music, 24, 18–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, E. (1999). Measuring emotion continuously: Validity and reliability of the two-dimensional emotion-space. Australian Journal of Psychology, 51(3), 154–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, E. (2007). Locus of emotion: The effect of task order and age on emotion perceived and emotion felt in response to music. Journal of Music Therapy, 44(4), 344–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, E. (2009–2010). The fundamental function of music. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 63–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, E. (2010). Affective, evaluative, and collative responses to hated and loved music. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 4(1), 36–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, E. (2013). Loved music can make a listener feel negative emotions. Musicae Scientiae, 17(1), 11–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, E. (2016). Enjoying sad music: Paradox or parallel processes? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 312. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, K. D., & Fouts, G. T. (2003). Music preferences, personality style, and developmental issues of adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence., 32(3), 205–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, R., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2008). Dissociative experience and cultural neuroscience: Narrative, metaphor and mechanism. Cultural Medical Psychiatry, 32, 31–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J., & Brown, E. M. (2007). Anger, disgust and the negative aesthetic emotions: Expanding an appraisal model of aesthetic experience. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 1(2), 100–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simeon, D., Guralnik, O., Knutelska, M., & Schmeidler, J. (2002). Personality factors associated with dissociation: Temperament, defense and cognitive schemata. American Journal of Psychology, 159, 489–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, M., & Lynn, S. J. (1989). Music, absorption and hypnotisability. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 37(1), 41–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tellegen, A. (1982). Brief manual for the multidimensional personality questionnaire. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (‘absorption’), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 268–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ter Bogt, T. F. M., Delsing, M. J. M. H., van Zalk, M., Christensen, P. G., & Meeus, W. H. J. (2011). Intergenerational continuity of taste: Parental and adolescent music preferences. Social Forces, 90(1), 297–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ter Bogt, T. F. M., Mulder, J., Raaijmakers, Q. A. W., & Gabhainn, S. N. (2010). Moved by music: A typology of music listeners. Psychology of Music, 39(2), 147–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(2), 284–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Schuengel, V. (1996). The measurement of dissociation in normal and clinical populations: Meta-analytic validation of the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES). Clinical Psychology Review, 16(5), 365–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vreeke, G. J., & Van der Mark, I. L. (2003). Empathy, an integrative model. New Ideas in Psychology, 21(3), 177–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vuoskoski, J. K. (2015). Music, empathy and affiliation: Commentary on Greenberg, Rentfrow, and Baron-Cohen. Empirical Musicology Review, 10(1), 99–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vuoskoski, J. K., Thompson, W. F., McIIlwain, D., & Eerola, T. (2012). Who enjoys listening to sad music and why? Music Perception, 29(3), 311–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walton, K. (1997). Listening with imagination: Is music representational? In J. Robinson (Ed.), Music and meaning. New York: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Westen, D. (2002). Psychology: Brain, behaviour and culture (3rd. ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wild, T. C., Kuiken, D., & Schopflocher, D. (1995). The role of absorption in experimental involvement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(3), 569–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Garrido, S. (2017). Individual Differences in the Attraction to Sad Music. In: Why Are We Attracted to Sad Music?. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-39666-8_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics