Skip to main content

Modelling Scientific Un/Certainty. Why Argumentation Strategies Trump Linguistic Markers Use

  • Conference paper
  • First Online:
Model-Based Reasoning in Science and Technology

Part of the book series: Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics ((SAPERE,volume 27))

Abstract

In recent years, there has been increasing interest in investigating science communication. Some studies that address this issue attempt to develop a model to determine the level of confidence that an author or a scientific community has at a given time towards a theory or a group of theories. A well-established approach suggests that, in order to determine the level of certainty authors have with regard to the statements they make, one can identify specific lexical and morphosyntactical markers which indicate their epistemic attitudes. This method is considered particularly appealing because it permits the development of an algorithmic model based on the quantitative analysis of the occurrence of these markers to assess (almost) automatically and objectively the opinion of an author or the predominant opinion of a scientific community on a topic at a given time. In this contribution we show that this line of research presents many kinds of problems especially when it is applied to research articles (rather than to popular science texts and basic research reports). To this aim, we propose two main lines of reasoning. The first one relies generally on the argumentative structure of scientific articles and shows that certainty/uncertainty markers are used differently in different argument forms and that therefore their number/frequency of use does not offer reliable indications for determining whether the topic at issue is considered by the authors to be more or less factual/speculative. The second one is based on the analysis of a sample of psychiatric research articles on homosexuality written over a long time span and taken from The British Journal of Psychiatry. Since the psychiatric perspective on homosexuality changed radically during the decades in which these articles were published, they offer an inventory of various kinds of argumentative strategies directed both at defending and confuting dominant as well as marginal positions. We focus especially on uncertainty markers and show that frequently the positions stated using expressions indicating uncertainty are actually not considered as conjectural or speculative by their authors, but that the use of uncertainty markers is motivated by a number of different and often incongruent rhetorical strategies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 259.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    For an overview on epistemicity and for a discussion of its affinity to evidentiality see e.g. Chafe and Nichols 1986, Bednarek 2006, Cornillie 2009. Even though most authors agree that these two notions are positively related to each other (that they overlap, or that epistemicity is included in/derived from evidentiality), the debate is not unanimous on this aspect: for a brief overview of the different ways in which the relationships between these two notions have been interpreted see e.g. Dendale and Tasmowski (2001).

  2. 2.

    Actually Hyland (1996b) calls this verb class ‘deductive’, but we prefer not to use this term since we consider it to be incorrect for the kind of verbs that he has in mind. In fact, he specifies that the verbs included in this class are related to inferential reasoning, while in another article he admits that science is mainly inductive: “In fact, my data indicates that few knowledge claims are presented in unmitigated form: induction and inference rather than deduction and causality characterize most arguments in scientific discourse” (Hyland 1996a: 435).

  3. 3.

    As examples of possible false results obtained identifying uncertain statements merely on the basis of uncertainty markers Agarwal and Yu (2010: 954) cite sentences like: “We can now study regulatory regions and functional domains of the protein in the context of a true erythroid environment, experiments that have not been possible heretofore.” In addition, they point out that—in the case of complex sentences in which only one part/aspect is qualified as uncertain through the association with a marker like “Right middle and (probable right lower) lobe pneumonia”—we need to be able to distinguish between the certain and the uncertain information (indicated in the example using the square brackets).

  4. 4.

    Even though reasons and causes are clearly not one and the same thing, from the point of view of the logical structure of the argument we can consider them as equal (see Sinott-Armstrong and Fogelin 2009: 3–16).

  5. 5.

    According to the Deductive Nomologic Model developed by Carl Gustav Hempel and Paul Hoppenheim this is the only appropriate way to develop scientific explanations. See Hempel and Hoppenheim 1948, Hempel 1965, Ladyman 2002.

  6. 6.

    See Harman 1965, Boyd 1991, Psillos 1999, Ladyman 2002.

  7. 7.

    From 1855 till 1858 the periodical was known as Asylum Journal and from 1858 to 1863 it took the name of Journal of Mental Science.

  8. 8.

    DSM-I was published in 1952; DSM-II appeared in 1968, but it underwent some important changes in the seventh printing of 1974; DSM-III was published in 1980 but in 1987 a revisited version (DSM-III-R) came out; DSM-IV appeared in 1994; a revisited version was then published in 2000. The fifth and last version of DSM was published in 2013.

  9. 9.

    Here we report the complete list of the titles and authors making up the sample. 1921 (Homosexuality, by C.S. Read); 1957 (Psychometric Aspects of Homosexuality, by T.G. Grygier); 1962 (Homosexuality and Genetic Sex by M. Pritchard); 1964 (Homosexuality in Twins: A Report on Three Discordant Pairs by N. Parker); 1965 (2 articles of the same author, On the Genesis of Female Homosexuality and On the Genesis of Male Homosexuality: An Attempt at Clarifying the Role of the Parents by E. Bene); 1968 (Studies in Female Homosexuality IV. Social and Psychiatric Aspects by F.E. Kenyon); 1969 (Parental Age of Homosexuals by K. Abe and P.A.P. Moran); 1969 (Aversion Therapy of Homosexuality. A pilot study of 10 cases by J. Bancroft); 1969 (Homosexuality, Exhibitionism and Fetishism-Transvestism. Some Experiences in the Use of Aversion Therapy in Male by B.H. Fookes); 1970 (Subjective and Penile Plethysmograph Responses to Aversion Therapy for Homosexuality: A Follow-up Study by N. McConaghy); 1971 (A Male Monozygotic Twinship Discordant for Homosexuality. A Repertory Grid Study by K. Davidson, H. Brierley and C. Smith); 1972 (Parent-Child Relationships and Homosexuality by G. Robertson); 1973 (Heterosexual Aversion in Homosexual Males) by K. Freund, R. Langevin, S. Cibiri and Y. Zajac; 1973 (Classical, Avoidance and Backward Conditioning Treatments of Homosexuality by N. McConaghy and R.F. Barr); 1973 (Doctors’ Attitudes to Homosexuality by P.A. Morris); 1974 (Sex Chromosome Abnormalities, Homosexuality and Psychological Treatment by A. Orwin, S.R.N. James and R.K. Turner); 1974 (Parental Background of Homosexual and Heterosexual Women by M. Siegelman); 1974 (Personality Characteristics of Male Homosexuals Referred for Aversion Therapy: A Comparative Study by R.K. Turner, H. Pielmaier, S. James and A. Orwin; 1980 (Social and Psychological Functioning of the Ageing Male Homosexual by K.C. Bennett and N.L. Thompson); 1980 (Homosexuality and Parental Guilt by B. Zuger); 1981 (Neuroendocrine Mechanisms and the Aetiology of Male and Female Homosexuality by M.J. MacCulloch and J.L. Waddington; 1983 (Homosexuality and Lesbianism by D.J.West); 1986 (Homosexuality in Monozygotic Twins Reared Apart by E.D. Eckert, T.J. Bouchard, J. Bohlen and L. Heston); 1999 (British psychiatry and homosexuality by M. King and A. Bartlett); 2001 (Straight talking: an investigation of the attitudes and practice of psychoanalysts and psychotherapists in relation to gays and lesbians by A. Bartlett, M. King and P. Phillips).

  10. 10.

    The square brackets do not indicate omitted text, we left out only supporting literature; bold indicates the hedges.

  11. 11.

    In the original article the author uses Arabic numerals (1)–(4) instead of Roman ones (i)–(iv). We changed this in order to distinguish more clearly between the points made by the authors and the numbers used to diagram the argument.

  12. 12.

    By “nuclear sex” the author means the sex as determined by using the presence or absence of sex chromatin in somatic cells. Its presence usually indicates the female genotype XX; while its absence indicates the male genotype XY.

  13. 13.

    While this was considered a pathology, a wide variety of techniques had been used in the treatment of homosexuality. Among them there were several types of “aversion therapies” which made use of various kinds of aversive stimuli to change the preference of so-called inverts towards heterosexuality. In this paper, the authors used an ‘anticipatory avoidance therapy’ analogous to that described by MacCulloch and Feldman (1967: 594). Basically, this is a technique similar to classical conditioning: the patient receives an electric shock when he is watching at pictures of attractive males.

  14. 14.

    In the text we left out the authors merely present further literature which confirms the point.

References

  • Afsar, H. S., Moradi, M., & Hamzavi, R. (2014). Frequency and type of hedging devices used in the research articles of humanities, basic sciences and agriculture procedia. Social and Behavioral Sciences, 136, 70–74. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.05.290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Agarwal, S., & Yu, H. (2010). Detecting hedge cues and their scope in biomedical text with conditional random fields. Journal of Biomedical Informatics, 43, 953–961. doi:10.1016/j.jbi.2010.08.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aguilar, M. (2008). Metadiscourse in academic speech: A relevance-theoretic approach. Berlin: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aikhenvald, A. Y. (2003a). Evidentiality in typological perspective. In A. Y. Aikhenvald (Ed.), Studies in evidentiality (pp. 1–31). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Aikhenvald, A. Y. (2003b). Preliminaries and key concepts. In A. Y. Aikhenvald (Ed.), Evidentiality (pp. 1–22). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Auger, A., & Roy, J. (2008). Expression of uncertainty in linguistic data. In Information Fusion, IEEE, 11th Conference on Information Fusion (pp 1860–1866).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bancroft, J. (1969). Aversion therapy of homosexuality: A pilot study of 10 cases. British Journal of Psychiatry, 115, 1417–14321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bednarek, M. (2006). Epistemological positioning and evidentiality in English news discourse: A text-driven approach. Text & Talk, 26(6), 635–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bene, E. (1965a). On the genesis of male homosexuality: An attempt at clarifying the role of parents. British Journal of Psychiatry, 111, 803–813.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bene, E. (1965b). On the genesis of female homosexuality. British Journal of Psychiatry, 111, 815–821.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beyer, R. (1987). Homosexuality and American psychiatry: The politics of diagnosis. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boas, F. (1938). Language. In F. Boas (Ed.), General anthropology (pp. 124–145). Boston/New York: D. C. Heath and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bongelli, R., et al. (2012). A corpus of scientific biomedical texts spanning over 168 years annotated for uncertainty. In N. Calzolari, K. Choukri, T. Declerck, M. Uğur Doğan, B. Maegaard, J. Mariani, A. Moreno, J. Odijk & S. Piperidis (Eds.), Proceedings of the Eight International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC) vol. 12 (pp. 2009–2014). Istanbul: European Language Resources Association—ELRA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bongelli, R., Riccioni, I., Canestrari, C., Pietrobon, R., & Zuczkowski, A. (2014). Biouncertainty: A historical corpus evaluating uncertainty language over a 167-year span of biomedical scientific articles. In A. Zuczkowski, R. Bongelli, I. Riccioni, & C. Canestrari (Eds.), Communicating certainty and uncertainty in medical, supportive and scientific contexts (pp. 309–339). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R. (1991). Explanation, explanatory power and semolicity. In R. Boyd, R. Gasper, & D. Trout (Eds.), The philosophy of science (pp. 349–377). Cambridge (MA): The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. [1978](1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W., & Nichols, J. (1986). Introduction. In W. Chafe & J. Nichols (Eds.) (vii–xi), Evidentiality: The linguistic coding of epistemology. Praeger (NJ): Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chao, F., & Hu, G. (2014). Interactive metadiscourse in research articles: A comparative study of paradigmatic and disciplinary influences. Journal of Pragmatics, 66, 15–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, E., Tan, C., Lee, L., Danescu-Miculescu-Mizil, C., & Spindel, J. (2012). Hedge detection as a lens on framing in the GMO debates: A position paper. In ACL-2012 Workshop on Extra-propositional Aspects of Meaning in Computational Linguistics (pp. 70–79).

    Google Scholar 

  • Coates, J. (1983). The semantics of the modal auxiliaries. London-Camberra: Croom Helm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coffin, C. J., Curry, M. J., Goodman, S., Hewings, A., Lillis, T., & Swann, J. (2003). Teaching academic writing: A toolkit for higher education. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornillie, B. (2009). Evidentiality and epistemic modality. On the close relationship between two different Categories. Functions of Language, 16(1), 44–62. doi:10.1075/fol.16.1.04cor.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crismore, A., & Farnsworth, R. (1990). Metadiscourse in popular and professional science discourse. In W. Nash (Ed.), The writing scholar: Studies in academic discourse (pp. 119–136). Newbury Park (CA): Stage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crismore, A., Markkanen, R., & Steffensen, M. S. (1993). Metadiscourse in persuasive writing. Written Communication, 10(1), 39–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • da Graça Pinto, M., Osorio, P., & Martins, F. (2014). A theoretical contribution to tackling certainty and uncertainty in scientific writing. In A. Zuczkowski, R. Bongelli, I. Riccioni, & C. Canestrari (Eds.), Communicating certainty and uncertainty in medical, supportive and scientific contexts (pp. 291–305). Amsterdam: John Benjamin (Four research articles from the journal Brain in focus).

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, K., Brierley, H. & Smith, C. (1971). A male monozygotic twinship discordant for homosexuality: A repertory grid study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 118, 675–682.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dendale, P., & Tasmowski, L. (2001). Introduction: Evidentiality and related notions. Journal of Pragmatics, 33, 339–348. doi:10.1016/S0378-2166(00)00005-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drescher, J., & Merlino, J. P. (2007). Introduction. In J. Drescher & J. P. Merlino (Eds.), American psychiatry and homosexuality: An oral history (pp. 1–11). New York/London: Harrington Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durik, A. M., Britt, M. A., Reynolds, R., & Storey, J. (2008). The effects of hedges in persuasive arguments. A nuanced analysis of language. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 27(3), 213–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farkas, R., Vincze, V., Móra, G., Csirik, J., & Szarvas, G. (2010). The CoNLL-2010 shared task: Learning to detect hedges and their scope in natural language text. In Proceedings of the 14th Conference on Computational Natural Language Learning: Shared task (pp. 1–12).

    Google Scholar 

  • Freund, K., Langevin, R., Cibiri, S. & Zayac, Y. (1973). Heterosexual aversion in homosexual males. British Journal of Psychiatry, 122, 163–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fookes, B.H. (1969). Some experiences in the use of aversion therapy in male homosexuality, exibitionism and fetishism-transvestitism. British Journal of Psychiatry, 115, 339–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, G. N., & Mulkay, M. (1984). Opening pandora’s box: A sociological analysis of scientific discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grygier, T. G. (1957). Psychometric aspects of homosexuality. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 103, 514–526. doi:10.1192/bjp.103.432.514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harman, G. H. (1965). Inference to the best explanation. Philosophical Review., 74(1), 88–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hempel, C. G. (1965). Aspects of scientific explanation. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hempel, C. G., & Hoppenheim, P. (1948). Studies in the logic of explanation. Philosophy of Science, 15(2), 135–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heritage, J. (2011). Territories of knowledge, territories of experience: Emphatic moments in interaction. In T. Stivers, L. Mondada, & J. Steensig (Eds.), The morality of knowledge in conversation (pp. 159–183). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (1983). Speaking English with the appropriate degree of conviction. In C. Brumfit (Ed.), Learning and teaching languages for communication: Applied linguistics perspectives (pp. 100–121). London: BAAL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (1984). Modifying illocutionary force. Journal of Pragmatics, 8, 345–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (1990). Hedges and boosters in women’s and men’s speech. Language & Communication, 10(3), 185–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (1995). Author in the text. Hedging scientific strategies. Hong Kong Papers in Linguistic and Language Teaching, 18, 33–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (1996a). Writing without conviction? Hedging in scientific research articles. Applied Linguistics, 17(4), 433–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (1996b). Talking to the academy: Forms of hedging in science research articles. Written Communication, 13(2), 251–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (1997). scientific claims and community values: Articulating an academic culture. Language & Communication, 17(1), 19–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (1999). Academic attribution: Citation and the construction of disciplinary knowledge. Applied Linguistics, 20(3), 341–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (2002). Authority and invisibility: Authorial identity in academic writing. Journal of Pragmatics, 34, 1091–1112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (2004). Disciplinary discourses: Social interactions in academic writing. USA: University of Michigan Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (2005). Metadiscourse. London: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (2006). Disciplinary differences: Language variations in academic discourses. In K. Hyland & M. Bondi (Eds.) Academic discourses across disciplines (pp. 17–45). Bern Frankfurt a.M. London New York: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (2009). Writing in the disciplines: Research evidence for specificity. Taiwan International ESP Journal, 1(1), 5–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (2011). Disciplines and discourses: Social interactions in the constructions of knowledge. In D. Starke-Meyerring, A. Paré, N. Artemeva, M. Horne, & L. Yousoubova (Eds.), Writing in the knowledge society (pp. 193–214). West Lafayette (IN): Parlor Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, K. (2012). Disciplinary identities: Individuality and community in academic discourse. Cambridge, (UK): Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ifantidou, E. (2005). The semantics and pragmatics of metadiscourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 1325–1353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamio, A. (1997). Territory of information. Amsterdam: John Benjamin.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kenyon, F.E. (1968). Studies in female homosexuality: IV. Social and psychiatric aspects. British Journal of Psychiatry, 114, 1337–1350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J.-D., Ohta, T., Pyysalo, S., Kano, Y., & Tsujii, J. (2009). Overview of BioNLP’09 shared task on event extraction. In Proceedings of the BioNLP 2009 Workshop (pp. 1–9). Boulder, Colorado.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiyavitskaya, N., Zeni, N., Cordy, J. R., Mich, L., & Mylopoulos, J. (2005). Semi-automatic semantic annotations for web documents. Proceedings of SWAP, 2005, 14–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knorr-Cetina, K. (1981). The manufacture of knowledge: An essay on the constructivist and contextual nature of science. Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladyman, J. (2002). Understanding philosophy of science. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1973). Hedges: A study in meaning criteria and the logic of fuzzy concepts. Journal of Philosophical Logic, 2, 458–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things. What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, T. (1940). Studies on the genetic determination of homosexuality. Journal of Nervous and Mental Desease, 92, 55–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B., & Woolgar, S. (1979). Laboratory life: The social construction of scientific facts. Beverly Hills (CA): Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemke, J. (1998). Multiplying meaning. Visual and verbal semiotics in scientific text. In J. R. Martin & R. Veel (Eds.), Reading science. Critical and functional perspectives on discourse of science (pp. 87–113). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liddicoat, A. J. (2005). Writing about knowing in science: Aspects of hedging in french scientific writing. LPS & Professional Communication, 5(2), 8–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacCulloch, M. J., & Feldman, M. P. (1967). Aversion therapy in management of 43 homosexuals. British Medical Journal, 2, 594–597.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McConaghy, N. (1970). Subjective and penile plethysmograph responses to aversion therapy for homosexuality: A follow-up study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 117, 555–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConaghy, N. & Barr, R.F. (1973). Classical, avoidance and backward conditioning treatments of homosexuality. British Journal of Psychiatry, 122, 151–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morante, R., & Daelemans, W. (2009). Learning the scope of hedge cues in biomedical texts. In Proceedings of the Workshop on BioNLP (pp. 28–36).

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, G. (1989). The pragmatics of politeness in scientific articles. Applied Linguistics, 10, 1–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Origgi, G., & Sperber, D. (2002). Evolution, communication, and the proper function of language. In P. Carruthers & A. Chamberlain (Eds.), Evolution and the human mind: Language, modularity, and social cognition (pp. 140–169). Cambridge (MA): Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orwin, A., James, S. R. N., & Turner, K. (1974). Sex chromosome abnormalities, homosexuality and psychological treatment. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 124, 293–295. doi:10.1192/bjp.124.3.293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porcher, J. E. (2014). A note on the dynamics of psychiatric classification. Minerva, 18, 27–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard, M. (1962). Homosexuality and genetic sex. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 108, 616–623. doi:10.1192/bjp.108.456.616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Psillos, S. (1999). Scientific realism. London New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosario, V. A. (2002). Homosexuality and science. A guide to debate. Santa Barbara (CA): ABC-CLIO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, S., Lipovsky, W. J., McKeown, K., Thadani, K., & Andreas, J. (2010). Towards semi-automated annotation for prepositional phrase attachment. In Proceedings of the 7th Conference on International Language Resources and Evaluation LREC 2010. http://www.lrec.conf.org.

  • Rubin, V. L., Liddy, E. D., & Kando, N. (2006). Certainty Identification in texts: Categorization model and manual tagging results. In J. G. Shanahan, Y. Qu, & J. Wiebe (Eds.), Computing attitude and affect in text: Theory and applications (pp. 61–76). Dordrecht (NL): Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Siegelman, M. (1974). Parental background of homosexual and heterosexual women. British Journal of Psychiatry, 124, 14–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinott-Armstrong, W., & Fogelin, R. J. (2009). Understanding arguments: An introduction to informal logic. Cengage: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skelton, J. (1997). The representation of truth in academic medical writing. Applied Linguistics, 18(2), 121–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szarvas, G., Vincze, V., Farkas, R., & Csirik, J. (2008). The Bioscope corpus: Annotation for negation, uncertainty and their scope in biomedical texts. Association for Computational Linguistics, BioNLP: Current Trends in Biomedical Natural Language Processing, 38–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terman, L.M. & Miles, C.C. (1936a). Attitude-interest analysis test. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terman, L.M. & Miles, C.C. (1936b). Sex and personality. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thagard, P. (1992). Conceptual revolutions. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, G. (2001). Interaction in academic writing: Learning to argue with the reader. Applied Linguistics, 22, 58–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vincze, V., Szarvas, G., Farkas, R., Móra, G., & Csirik, J. (2008). The bio-scope corpus: Biomedical texts annotated for uncertainty. Negation and their Scopes. BMC Bioinformatics, 9(11), S9. doi:10.1186/1471-2105-9-S11-S9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. (2000). Metarepresentation in linguistic communication. In D. Sperber (Ed.), Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 411–448). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. (2005). New directions for research on pragmatics and modularity. Lingua, 115, 1129–1146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, S., Krämer, N. C., Rösner, L., & Neubaum, G. (2015). Don’t keep it (too) simple—How textual representations of scientific uncertainty affet layperson’ attitude. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 34(3), 251–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments. This research was supported by the ORBA10H82A grant to LP and by the 40201691 grant to SD.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Luigi Pastore .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this paper

Cite this paper

Pastore, L., Dellantonio, S. (2016). Modelling Scientific Un/Certainty. Why Argumentation Strategies Trump Linguistic Markers Use. In: Magnani, L., Casadio, C. (eds) Model-Based Reasoning in Science and Technology. Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics, vol 27. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-38983-7_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics