Skip to main content

The Relevance of Context and Experience for the Operation of Historical Sound Change

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Toward Robotic Socially Believable Behaving Systems - Volume II

Part of the book series: Intelligent Systems Reference Library ((ISRL,volume 106))

Abstract

This paper is concerned with explaining how historical sound change can emerge as a consequence of the association between continuous, dynamic speech signals and phonological categories. The relevance of this research to developing socially believable speech processing machines is that sound change is both cognitive and social and also because it provides a unique insight into how the categories of speech and language and dynamic speech signals are inter-connected. A challenge is to understand how unstable conditions that can lead to sound change are connected with the more typical stable conditions in which sound change is minimal. In many phonetic models of sound change, stability and instability come about because listeners typically parse—very occasionally misparse—overlapping articulatory movements in a way that is consistent with their production. Experience-based models give greater emphasis to how interacting individuals can bring about sound change at the population level. Stability in these models is achieved through reinforcing in speech production the centroid of a probability distribution of perceived episodes that give rise to a phonological category; instability and change can be brought about under various conditions that cause different category distributions to shift incrementally and to come into contact with each other. Beyond these issues, the natural tendency to imitation in speech communication may further incrementally contribute to sound change both over adults’ lifespan and in the blending of sounds that can arise through dialect contact. The general conclusion is that the instabilities that give rise to sound change are an inevitable consequence of the same mechanisms that are deployed in maintaining the stability between phonological categories and their association with the speech signal.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  1. Abrego-Collier C (2013) Liquid dissimilation as listener hypocorrection. In: Proceedings of the 37th annual meeting of the Berkeley linguistics society, 3–17

    Google Scholar 

  2. Alderete J, Frisch S (2006) Dissimilation in grammar and the lexicon. In: de Lacy P (ed) The cambridge handbook of phonology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  3. Aylett M, Turk A (2004) The smooth signal redundancy hypothesis: a functional explanation for relationships between redundancy, prosodic prominence, and duration in spontaneous speech. Lang Speech 47:31–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Babel M (2010) Dialect divergence and convergence in New Zealand English. Lang Soc 39:437–456

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Babel M (2012) Evidence for phonetic and social selectivity in spontaneous phonetic imitation. J Phon 40:177–189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Babel M, McGuire G, Walters S, Nicholls A (2014) Novelty and social preference in phonetic accommodation. Lab Phonol 5:123–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Baker A, Archangeli D, Mielke J (2011) Variability in American English s-retraction suggests a solution to the actuation problem. Lang Var Chang 23:347–374

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Baxter G, Blythe R, Croft W, McKane A (2009) Modeling language change: an evaluation of Trudgill’s theory of the emergence of New Zealand English. Lang Var Chang 21:257–293

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Beckman M, Edwards J (1994) Articulatory evidence for differentiating stress categories. In: Keating P (ed) Papers in laboratory phonology III. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 7–33

    Google Scholar 

  10. Beckman M, Munson B, Edwards JR (2007) Vocabulary growth and the developmental expansion of types of phonological knowledge. In: Hualde J, Cole J (eds) Papers in laboratory phonology IX. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 241–264

    Google Scholar 

  11. Beckman M, Li F, Kong E, Edwards J (2014) Aligning the timelines of phonological acquisition and change. Lab Phonol 5:151–194

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Beddor P (2009) A coarticulatory path to sound change. Language 85:785–821

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Beddor P (2012) Perception grammars and sound change. In: Solé M-J, Recasens D (eds) The initiation of sound change. Perception, production, and social factors. John Benjamin, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  14. Beddor PS, Krakow RA (1999) Perception of coarticulatory nasalization by speakers of English and Thai: evidence for partial compensation. J Acoust Soc Am 106:2868–2887

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Blevins J (2004) Evolutionary phonology: the emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  16. Blevins J, Garrett A (2004) The evolution of metathesis. In: Hayes B, Kirchner R, Steriade D (eds) Phonetically-based phonology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 117–156

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  17. Blevins J, Wedel A (2009) Inhibited sound change: an evolutionary approach to lexical competition. Diachronica 26:143–183

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Bloomfield L (1933) Language. Holt, New York

    Google Scholar 

  19. Boersma P, Hamann S (2008) The evolution of auditory dispersion in bidirectional constraint grammars. Phonology 25:217–270

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Bradlow AR (2002) Confluent talker-and listener-oriented forces in clear speech production. In: Gussenhoven C, Warner N (eds) Papers in laboratory phonology VII. Mouton de Gruyter, New York, pp 241–273

    Google Scholar 

  21. Browman C, Goldstein L (1992) Articulatory phonology: an overview. Phonetica 49:155–180

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Bukmaier V, Harrington J, Kleber F (2014) An analysis of post-vocalic /s-\(\int \)/ neutralization in Augsburg German: evidence for a gradient sound change. Front. Psychol 5:1–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Bybee J (2001) Phonology and language use. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  24. Bybee J (2002) Word frequency and context of use in the lexical diffusion of phonetically conditioned sound change. Lang Va Chang 14:261–290

    Google Scholar 

  25. Castellano C, Fortunato S, Loreto V (2009) Statistical physics of social dynamics. Rev Mod Phys 81:591–646

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Chambers J (1992) Dialect acquisition. Language 68:673–705

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Chen M (1970) Vowel length variation as a function of the voicing of the consonant environment. Phonetica 22:129–159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Chen M, Wang W (1975) Sound change: actuation and implementation. Language 51:255–281

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Delvaux V, Soquet A (2007) The influence of ambient speech on adult speech productions through unintentional imitation. Phonetica 64:145–173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Diessel H (2012) Language change and language acquisition. In: Bergs A, Brinton L (eds) Historical linguistics of English: an international handbook, vol 2. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, pp 1599–1613

    Google Scholar 

  31. Docherty G, Foulkes P (2014) An evaluation of usage-based approaches to the modelling of sociophonetic variability. Lingua 142:42–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Egurtzegi A (2014) Towards a phonetically grounded diachronic phonology of Basque. PhD dissertation, University of the Basque Country

    Google Scholar 

  33. Fagyal Z, Escobar A, Swarup S, Gasser L, Lakkaraju K (2010) Centers and periph- eries: network roles in language change. Lingua 120:2061–2079

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Foulkes P, Vihman MM (in press) Language acquisition and phonological change. In: Honeybone P, Salmons JC (eds) The handbook of historical phonology. OUP, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  35. Foulkes P, Docherty G, Watt D (2005) Phonological variation in child directed speech. Language 81:177–206

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Fowler C (1984) Segmentation of coarticulated speech in perception. Percept Psychophys 36:359–368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Fowler C, Brown J (2000) Perceptual parsing of acoustic consequences of velum lowering from information for vowels. Percept Psychophys 62:21–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Fowler C, Housum J (1987) Talkers’ signaling of ‘new’ and ‘old’ words in speech and listeners’ perception and use of the distinction. J Mem Lang 26:489–504

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Fowler C, Smith M (1986) Speech perception as “vector analysis”: an approach to the problems of segmentation and invariance. In: Perkell J, Klatt D (eds) Invariance and variability in speech processes. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 123–139

    Google Scholar 

  40. Fowler C, Richardson M, Marsh K, Shockley K (2008) Language use, coordination, and the emergence of cooperative action. In: Fuchs A, Jirsa V (eds) Understanding complex systems. Springer, Berlin, pp 261–279

    Google Scholar 

  41. Fujisaki H, Kunisaki O (1976) Analysis, recognition and perception of voiceless fricative consonants in Japanese. Annu Bull Res Inst Logop Phoniatr 10:145–156

    Google Scholar 

  42. Garrett A, Johnson K (2013) Phonetic bias in sound change. In: Yu A (ed) Origins of sound change. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 51–97

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  43. Garrod S, Pickering M (2009) Joint action, interactive alignment, and dialog. Top Cogn Sci 1:292–304

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Goldinger S (1997) Words and voices: perception and production in an episodic lexicon. In: Johnson K, Mullennix J (eds) Talker variability in speech processing. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 33–66

    Google Scholar 

  45. Grammont M (1902) Observations sur le langage des enfants. In Me langes Linguistiques Offerts aI M. Antoine Meillet. Klincksieck, Paris, pp 115–131

    Google Scholar 

  46. Greenlee M, Ohala J (1980) Phonetically motivated parallels between child phonology and historical sound change. Lang Sci 2:283–301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Grosvald M, Corina D (2012) The production and perception of sub-phonemic vowel contrasts and the role of the listener in sound change. In: Solé M-J, Recasens D (eds) The initiation of sound change. Perception, production, and social factors. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 77–100

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  48. Guion S (1998) The role of perception in the sound change of velar palatalization. Phonetica 55:18–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Halle M, Hughes G, Radley J-P (1957) Acoustic properties of stop consonants. J Acoust Soc Am 29:107–116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Harrington J (2006) An acoustic analysis of ‘happy-tensing’ in the Queen’s Christmas broadcasts. J Phon 34:439–457

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Harrington J (2010) Acoustic phonetics. In: Hardcastle W, Laver J, Gibbon F (eds) A handbook of phonetics. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford, pp 81–129

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  52. Harrington J (2012) The relationship between synchronic variation and diachronic change. In: Cohn A, Fougeron C, Huffman M (eds) Handbook of laboratory phonology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 321–332

    Google Scholar 

  53. Harrington J, Palethorpe S, Watson C (2000) Does the Queen speak the Queen’s English? Nature 408:927–928

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Harrington J, Kleber F, Reubold U (2008) Compensation for coarticulation, /u/-fronting, and sound change in Standard Southern British: an acoustic and perceptual study. J Acoust Soc Am 123:2825–2835

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Harrington J, Hoole P, Kleber F, Reubold U (2011) The physiological, acoustic, and perceptual basis of high back vowel fronting: evidence from German tense and lax vowels. J Phon 39:121–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Harrington J, Kleber F, Reubold U (2012) The production and perception of coarticulation in two types of sound change in progress. In: Fuchs S, Weirich M, Perrier P, Pape D (eds) Speech production and speech perception: planning and dynamics. Peter Lang, Bern, pp 33–55

    Google Scholar 

  57. Harrington J, Kleber F, Reubold U (2013) The effect of prosodic weakening on the production and perception of trans-consonantal vowel coarticulation in German. J Acoust Soc Am 134:551–561

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Harrington J, Kleber F, Stevens M (2015) The relationship between the (mis)-parsing of coarticulation in perception and sound change: evidence from dissimilation and language acquisition. In: Esposito A, Faundez-Zany M (eds) Recent advances in nonlinear speech processing. Springer, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  59. Harrington J, Kleber F, Reubold U, Siddins J (2015) The implications for sound change of prosodic weakening: evidence from polysyllabic shortening. Lab Phonol 6(1):87–117

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Hawkins S (2003) Roles and representations of systematic fine phonetic detail in speech understanding. J Phon 31:373–405

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Hay J, Foulkes P (in press) The evolution of medial /t/ over real and remembered time. Language

    Google Scholar 

  62. Hay J, Jannedy S, Mendoza-Denton N (1999) Oprah and /ay/: lexical frequency, referee design and style. In: Proceedings of the 14th international congress of phonetic sciences, San Francisco

    Google Scholar 

  63. Hay J, Pierrehumbert J, Walker A, LaShell P (2015) Tracking word frequency effects through 130 years of sound change. Cognition 139:83–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Heid S, Hawkins S (2000) An acoustical study of long domain /r/ and /l/ coarticulation. In: Proceedings of the 5th seminar on speech production: models and data. Kloster Seeon, Bavaria, Germany. Munich, pp 77–80

    Google Scholar 

  65. Hintzman DL (1986) ‘Schema abstraction’ in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychol Rev 93:328–338

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Hockett C (1950) Age-grading and linguistic continuity. Language 26:449–457

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Hoole P, Pouplier M, Benus S, Bombien L (2013) Articulatory coordination in obstruent-sonorant clusters and syllabic consonants: data and modelling. In: Spreafico L, Vietti A (eds) Rhotics: new data and perspectives. Bolzano University Press, pp 81–97

    Google Scholar 

  68. Hualde J (2011) Sound change. In: van Oostendorp M, Ewen C, Hume E, Rice K (eds) The blackwell companion to phonology, vol IV. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford, pp 2214–2235

    Google Scholar 

  69. Hyman LM (2013) Enlarging the scope of phonologization. In: Yu A (ed) Origins of sound change: approaches to phonologization. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 3–28

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  70. Jakobson R (1941) Kindersprache, Aphasie, und allgemeine Lautgesetze. Almqvist & Wiksell, Uppsala

    Google Scholar 

  71. Janda R (2003) “Phonologization” as the start of dephoneticization—or, one sound change and its aftermath: of extension, generalization, lexicalization, and morphologization. In: Joseph B, Janda R (eds) The handbook of historical linguistics. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 401–422

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  72. Janda R, Joseph B (2003) Reconsidering the canons of sound-change: towards a “big bang” theory. In: Blake B, Burridge K (eds) Selected papers from the 15th international conference on historical linguistics. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 205–219

    Google Scholar 

  73. Johnson K (1997) Speech perception without speaker normalization: An exemplar model. In: Johnson K, Mullennix J (eds) Talker variability in speech processing. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 145–165

    Google Scholar 

  74. Kataoka R (2011) Phonetic and cognitive bases of sound change. Ph.D diss. University of California, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

  75. Kawasaki H (1986) Phonetic explanation for phonological universals: the case of distinctive vowel nasalization. In: Ohala J, Jaeger J (eds) Experimental phonology. Academic Press, Orlando, pp 81–103

    Google Scholar 

  76. Kerswill P (1996) Children, adolescents, and language change. Language Variation and Change 8:177–202

    Google Scholar 

  77. Kingston J, Diehl RL (1994) Phonetic knowledge. Language 70:419–454

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Kirby J (2013) The role of probabilistic enhancement in phonologization. In: Yu A (ed) Origins of sound change: approaches to phonologization. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 228–246

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  79. Kirby J (2014) Incipient tonogenesis in Phnom Penh Khmer: computational studies. Laboratory phonology 5:195–230

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  80. Kiparsky P (2008) Universals constrain change; change results in typological generalizations. In: Good J (ed) Linguistic universals and language change. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 23–53

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  81. Kleber F, Peters S (2014) Children’s imitation of coarticulatory patterns in different prosodic contexts. In: Proceedings of 14th conference on laboratory phonology, Tokyo, Japan

    Google Scholar 

  82. Kleber F, Harrington J, Reubold U (2012) The relationship between the perception and production of coarticulation during a sound change in progress. Lang Speech 55:383–405

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Labov W (1981) Resolving the Neogrammarian controversy. Language 57:267–308

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Labov W (1990) The intersection of sex and social class in the course of linguistic change. Lang Var Chang 2:205–254

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Labov W (1994) Principles of linguistic change. Volume 1 internal factors. Blackwell, Malden

    Google Scholar 

  86. Labov W (2001) Principles of linguistic change. Volume 2: social factors. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  87. Labov W (2007) Transmission and diffusion. Language 83:344–387

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Labov W (2010) Principles of linguistic change, Volume 3: cognitive and cultural factors. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  89. Lehiste I (1970) Suprasegmentals. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  90. Lightfoot D (1999) The development of language: acquisition, change, and evolution. Blackwell, Malden

    Google Scholar 

  91. Lin S, Beddor P, Coetzee A (2014) Gestural reduction, lexical frequency, and sound change: a study of post-vocalic /l/. Lab Phonol 5:9–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Lindblom B (1990) Explaining phonetic variation: a sketch of the H & H theory. In: Hardcastle W, Marchal A (eds) Speech production and speech modeling. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 403–439

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  93. Lindblom B, Studdert-Kennedy M (1967) On the role of formant transitions in vowel recognition. J Acoust Soc Am 42:830–843

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Lindblom B, Guion S, Hura S, Moon S-J, Willerman R (1995) Is sound change adaptive? Rivista di Linguistica 7:5–36

    Google Scholar 

  95. Lobanov B (1971) Classification of Russian vowels spoken by different speakers. J Acoust Soc Am 49:606–608

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Local J (2003) Variable domains and variable relevance: interpreting phonetic exponents. J Phon 31:321–329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Luick K (1964) Historische Grammatik der englischen Sprache. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  98. Maclagan M, Hay J (2007) Getting fed up with our feet: contrast maintenance and the New Zealand English “short” front vowel shift. Lang Var Chang 9:1–25

    Google Scholar 

  99. Mann V, Repp B (1980) Influence of vocalic context on the perception of [\(\int \)]–[s] distinction: I. Temporal factors. Percept Psychophys 28:213–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Martinet A (1955) Economie des Changements Phone tiques. Francke, Bern

    Google Scholar 

  101. Matthies M, Perrier P, Perkell JS, Zandipour M (2001) Variation in anticipatory coarticulation with changes in clarity and rate. J Speech Lang Hear Res 44:340–353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Mowrey R, Pagliuca W (1995) The reductive character of articulatory evolution. Rivista di Linguistica 7:37–124

    Google Scholar 

  103. Munson B, Beckman M, Edwards J (2012) Abstraction and specificity in early lexical representations: climbing the ladder of abstraction. In: Cohn A, Fougeron C, Huffman M (eds) The oxford handbook of laboratory phonology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 288–309

    Google Scholar 

  104. Müller D (2013) Liquid dissimilation with a special regard to Latin. In: Sánchez Miret F, Recasens D (eds) Studies in phonetics, phonology and sound change in romance. Lincom, Munich, pp 95–109

    Google Scholar 

  105. Müller V, Harrington J, Kleber F, Reubold U (2011) Age-dependent differences in the neutralization of the intervocalic voicing contrast: evidence from an apparent-time study on East Franconian. Interspeech, Florence

    Google Scholar 

  106. Nielsen K (2011) Specificity and abstractness of VOT imitation. J Phon 39:132–142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  107. Nielsen K (2014) Phonetic imitation by young children and its developmental changes. J Speech Lang Hear Res 57:2065–2075

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Nittrouer S, Studdert-Kennedy M (1987) The role of coarticulatory effects in the perception of fricatives by children and adults. J Speech Hear Res 30:319–329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Nosofsky R (1988) Exemplar-based accounts of relations between classification, recognition, and typicality. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 14:700–708

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Öhman S (1966) Coarticulation in VCV utterances: spectrographic measurements. J Acoust Soc Am 39:151–168

    Article  Google Scholar 

  111. Ohala J (1981) The listener as a source of sound change. In: Masek CS, Hendrick RA, Miller MF (eds) Papers from the parasession on language and behavior. Chicago Linguistic Society, Chicago, pp 178–203

    Google Scholar 

  112. Ohala J (1993) The phonetics of sound change. In: Jones C (ed) Historical linguistics: problems and perspectives. Longman, London, pp 237–278

    Google Scholar 

  113. Ohala J (2012) The listener as a source of sound change: an update. In: Solé M-J, Recasens D (eds) The initiation of sound change. perception, production, and social factors. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 21–36

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  114. Ohala M, Ohala J (1992) Phonetic universals and Hindi segment duration. In: Proceedings of the international conference on spoken language processing. Edmonton, Alberta, pp 831–834

    Google Scholar 

  115. Osthoff H, Brugmann K (1878) Morphologische Untersuchungen auf dem Gebiete der indogermanischen Sprachen, Band I. Leipzig

    Google Scholar 

  116. Pardo J, Jay I, Krauss R (2010) Conversational role influences speech imitation. Atten Percept Psychophys 72:2254–2264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  117. Pardo J, Gibbons R, Suppes A, Krauss R (2012) Phonetic convergence in college roommates. J Phon 40:190–197

    Article  Google Scholar 

  118. Paul H (1886) Prinzipien der Sprachgeschichte, 2nd edn. Halle, Niemeyer

    Google Scholar 

  119. Phillips B (2006) Word frequency and lexical diffusion. Palgrave Macmillan, Basingstoke

    Book  Google Scholar 

  120. Pierrehumbert J (2001) Exemplar dynamics: word frequency, lenition, and contrast. In: Bybee J, Hopper P (eds) Frequency effects and the emergence of lexical structure. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 137–157

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  121. Pierrehumbert J (2003) Phonetic diversity, statistical learning, and acquisition of phonology. Lang Speech 46:115–154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  122. Pierrehumbert J (2006) The next toolkit. J Phon 34:516–530

    Article  Google Scholar 

  123. Pierrehumbert J, Beckman M, Ladd DR (2000) Conceptual foundations of phonology as a laboratory science. In: Burton-Roberts N, Carr P, Docherty G (eds) Phonological knowledge. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 273–303

    Google Scholar 

  124. Pierrehumbert J, Stonedahl F, Dalaud R (2014) A model of grassroots changes in linguistic systems. arXiv:1408.1985v1

  125. Quené H (2013) Longitudinal trends in speech tempo: the case of Queen Beatrix. J Acoust Soc Am 133:452–457

    Article  Google Scholar 

  126. Repp BH (1982) Phonetic trading relations and context effects: new experimental evidence for a speech mode of perception. Psychol Bull 92:81–110

    Article  Google Scholar 

  127. Reubold U, Harrington J (In press) The influence of age on estimating sound change acoustically from longitudinal data. In: Wagner SE, Buchstaller I (eds) Panel studies of variation and change. Routledge Ltd, New York

    Google Scholar 

  128. Ruch H, Harrington J (2014) Synchronic and diachronic factors in the change from pre-aspiration to post-aspiration in Andalusian Spanish. J Phon 45:12–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  129. Sancier M, Fowler C (1997) Gestural drift in a bilingual speaker of Brazilian Portuguese and English. J Phon 25:421–436

    Article  Google Scholar 

  130. Sankoff G, Blondeau H (2007) Language change across the lifespan: /r/ in Montreal Speech. Language 83:560–588

    Article  Google Scholar 

  131. Schuchardt H (1885) Über die Lautgesetze: Gegen die Junggrammatiker. Oppenheim, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  132. Sebanz N, Bekkering H, Knoblich G (2006) Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn Sci 10:70–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  133. Shockley K, Richardson D, Dale R (2009) Conversation and coordinative structures. Top Cogn Sci 1:305–319

    Article  Google Scholar 

  134. Siddins J, Harrington J, Reubold U, Kleber F (2014) Investigating the relationship between accentuation, vowel tensity and compensatory shortening. In: Proceedings of 7th speech prosody conference. Dublin, Ireland

    Google Scholar 

  135. Silverman D (2006) the diachrony of labiality in trique, and the functional relevance of gradience and variation. In: Goldstein L, Whalen D, Best C (eds) Papers in laboratory phonology 8. New Haven, Mouton de Gruyter, pp 133–154

    Google Scholar 

  136. Solé M (2007) Controlled and mechanical properites in speech: a review of the literature. In: Solé M-J, Beddor P, Ohala M (eds) Experimental approaches to phonology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 302–321

    Google Scholar 

  137. Solé M (2009) Acoustic and aerodynamic factors in the interaction of features: the case of nasality and voicing. In: Vigário M, Frota S, Freitas MJ (eds) Phonetics and phonology: interactions and interrelations. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 205–234

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  138. Solé M (2014) The perception of voice-initiating gestures. Lab Phonol 5:37–68

    Article  Google Scholar 

  139. Stanford J, Kenny L (2013) Revisiting transmission and diffusion: an agent-based model of vowel chain shifts across large communities. Lang Var Chang 25:119–153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  140. Stevens K (1972) The quantal nature of speech: evidence from articulatory-acoustic data. In: David E, Denes P (eds) Human communication: a unified view. McGraw-Hill, New York, pp 51–66

    Google Scholar 

  141. Stevens K (1989) On the quantal nature of speech. J Phon 17:3–45

    Google Scholar 

  142. Stevens M, Harrington J (2014) The individual and the actuation of sound change. Loquens 1(1):e003. doi:10.3989/loquens.2014.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  143. Stevens M, Reubold U (2014) Pre-aspiration, quantity, and sound change. Lab Phonol 5:455–488

    Article  Google Scholar 

  144. Studdert-Kennedy M (1998) Introduction: the emergence of phonology. In: Hurford J, Studdert- Kennedy M, Knight C (eds) Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 169–176

    Google Scholar 

  145. Stampe D (1969) The acquisition of phonetic representation. In: Papers from the fifth regional meeting of the chicago linguistic society, pp 443–454

    Google Scholar 

  146. Sweet H (1888) A history of English sounds. Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  147. Torreira F (2012) Investigating the nature of aspirated stops in Western Andalusian Spanish. J Int Phon Assoc 42:49–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  148. Trudgill P (1974) Linguistic change and diffusion: description and explanation in sociolinguistic dialect geography. Lang Soc 3:215–246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  149. Trudgill P (1983) On dialect: social and geographical perspectives. New York University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  150. Trudgill P (2004) Dialect contact and new-dialect formation: the inevitability of colonial englishes. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh

    Google Scholar 

  151. Trudgill P (2008) Colonial dialect contact in the history of European languages: on the irrelevance of identity to new-dialect formation. Lang Soc 37:241–254

    Google Scholar 

  152. Trudgill P (2008) On the role of children, and the mechanical view: a rejoinder. Lang Soc 37:277–280

    Google Scholar 

  153. Vihman M (1980) Sound change and child language. In: Traugott E, La Brum R, Shepherd S (eds) Papers from the 4th international conference on historical linguistics. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 303–320

    Google Scholar 

  154. Wang WS-Y (1969) Competing changes as a cause of residue. Language 45:9–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  155. Wardhaugh R, Fuller J (2015) An introduction to sociolinguistics, 7th edn. Wiley Blackwell, Chichester

    Google Scholar 

  156. Watson CI, Maclagan M, Harrington J (2000) Acoustic evidence for vowel change in New Zealand English. Lang Var Chang 12:51–68

    Article  Google Scholar 

  157. Wedel A (2007) Feedback and regularity in the lexion. Phonology 24:147–185

    Article  Google Scholar 

  158. Weinreich U, Labov W, Herzog M (1968) Empirical foundations for a theory of language change. In: Lehmann W, Malkiel Y (eds) Directions for historical linguistics. University of Texas Press, Austin, pp 95–195

    Google Scholar 

  159. Wells J (1982) Accents of English 2: the British Isles. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  160. Wolfram W, Schilling-Estes N (2003) Dialectology and diffusion. In: Joseph B, Janda R (eds) The handbook of historical linguistics. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 713–735

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  161. Wright R (2003) Factors of lexical competition in vowel articulation. In: Local J, Ogden R, Temple R (eds) Papers in laboratory phonology VI. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 75–87

    Google Scholar 

  162. Yu A (2013) Individual differences in socio-cognitive processing and the actuation of sound change. In: Yu A (ed) Origins of sound change: approaches to phonologization. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 201–227

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  163. Yu A, Abrego-Collier C, Sonderegger M (2013) Phonetic imitation from an individual-difference perspective: Subjective attitude, personality, and ‘autistic’ traits. Plos One 8(9):e74746

    Article  Google Scholar 

  164. Zellou G, Tamminga M (2014) Nasal coarticulation changes over time in Philadelphia English. J Phon 47:18–35

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by European Research Council grant number 295573 ‘Sound change and the acquisition of speech’ (2012–2017). We are very grateful to Daniela Müller, two anonymous reviewers, and the editors for very helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jonathan Harrington .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Harrington, J., Kleber, F., Reubold, U., Stevens, M. (2016). The Relevance of Context and Experience for the Operation of Historical Sound Change. In: Esposito, A., Jain, L. (eds) Toward Robotic Socially Believable Behaving Systems - Volume II . Intelligent Systems Reference Library, vol 106. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-31053-4_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-31053-4_6

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-31052-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-31053-4

  • eBook Packages: EngineeringEngineering (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics