Skip to main content

Biological Basis of Behaviour

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 1238 Accesses

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Ethics ((BRIEFSETHIC))

Abstract

With the exception of the most involuntary of reflexes, my actions are the consequence of the processes going on within my brain. In this chapter, our focus switches to scientific approaches which offer insight into the underlying biology of behaviour. Findings emerging from recent developments in both genetics and neuroscience are considered. The strengths and weaknesses of a range of methodologies used for imaging the brain are reviewed, with particular attention given to the roles played by Electroencephalography, Positron Emission Tomography and functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in the study of operational brains. We reflect on the implications for “free will” of a series of experiments which appear to demonstrate brain activity prior to conscious awareness of decision. Validated examples of genes involved in behaviour are discussed, alongside the growing importance of an understanding of the molecular mechanism by which expression of “hard wired” genes can be moderated by environmental stimuli.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   64.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    A distinction is drawn between “genomics” in which a significant number of different genes are investigated at the same time (possibly the entire genetic make-up of an individual) as against “genetics” where the focus is more likely to be on relationship with one or two specific genes.

  2. 2.

    It is important to distinguish between different uses of the term “reductionist”. For example, reductionism as a research methodology, in which large problems are broken down into more manageable and testable questions, remains a profoundly valuable tool for the conduct of much science. In contrast, the kind of ontological or causal reductionism, represented here by Crick, is a philosophical position offering (over)simplistic explanations of the functioning of life.

  3. 3.

    Addition, in order, of a methyl ( –CH3 group), acetyl group ( –COCH3), a 76 amino acid ubiquitin molecule, or an approximately 100 amino acid Small Ubiqitin-like MOdifier. The details of these processes is irrelevant to the current argument.

  4. 4.

    Also known as norepinephrine.

  5. 5.

    By convention the abbreviations for proteins are given in regular text, whereas the abbreviations for genes are put in italics.

  6. 6.

    Replacement of the usual cytosine (C) by a thymine (T) base at position 936 in the coding sequence.

  7. 7.

    Females have two X chromosomes; mutation on one copy may be compensated for by a functional copy on the other X chromosome. Because males have one X and one Y chromosome, they only have one copy of genes on the X chromosome and therefore any deficiency will be manifest.

  8. 8.

    The researchers were intentionally mutating the mouse genome, but via the relatively random mechanism of transposon mutagenesis. The fact that they hit upon the MAOA gene was providential and not their initial goal.

  9. 9.

    That is to say a long-term investigation of a group, attempting to correlate their experiences with outcomes, without pre-determining the interventions they will encounter.

  10. 10.

    Study of CpG methylation was actually conducted using white blood cells, a validated proxy for methylation in the brain. MAOA activity within the brain was monitored via the metabolism of a radioactively-labelled substrate clorgyline, which is only processed by this enzyme. PET was used to record the levels of radioactivity.

  11. 11.

    Despite the interpretations made within the research articles by Libet himself, and by countless others examining the significance of his work, Libet still wishes to retain the potential for free will to be real. In an essay published in 2011, he summarises his position thus: “My conclusion about free will, one genuinely free in the nondetermined sense, is then that its existence is at least as good, if not a better, scientific option than is its denial by determinist theory. Given the speculative nature of both determinist and nondeterminist theories, why not adopt the view that we do have free will (until some real contradictory evidence may appear, if it ever does)” (Libet 2011: p 9).

  12. 12.

    First use of the term “free won’t” is generally attributed to Vilayanar Ramachandran but has readily been adopted by Libet and others.

  13. 13.

    The authors are aware of the potential criticism of any experiment with “negative findings” that it may simply not have worked properly.

References

  • Alper, J.S. 1998. Genes, free will and criminal responsibility. Social Science and Medicine 46: 1599–1611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, D.A., and J. Bonta. 2010. The psychology of criminal conduct, 5th ed. New Providence, NJ: Matthew Bender & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balleine, B.W., and S. Killcross. 2006. Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function. Trends in Neurosciences 29: 272–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, M.L. 2013. The monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) genetic predisposition to impulsive violence: is it relevant to criminal cases? Neuroethics 6: 287–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, A. 2002. Magnetic resonance imaging. British Medical Journal 324: 35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, A. 2007. Perceptions of epigenetics. Nature 447: 396–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brierley, B., P. Shaw, and A.S. David. 2002. The human amydala: a systematic review and meta-analysis of volumetric magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Research Reviews 39: 84–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brogdon, B.G., and J.E. Lichtenstein. 1998. Forensic radiology in historical perspective. In Forensic radiology, ed. B.G. Brogdon, 18–32. Boca Raton, USA: CRC Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brunner, H.G., M. Nelen, X.O. Breakefield, H.H. Ropers, and B.A. van Oost. 1993. Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science 262: 578–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckholtz, J.W., and A. Meyer-Lindenberg. 2008. MAOA and the neurogenetic architecture of human aggression. Trends in Neurosciences 31: 120–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bunge, S.A., K.N. Ochsner, J.E. Desmond, G.H. Glover, and J.D.E. Gabrieli. 2001. Prefrontal regions involved in keeping information in and out of mind. Brain 124: 2074–2086.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burns, A., R. Jacoby, and R. Levy. 1990. Psychiatric phenomena in Alzheimer’s disease. IV: Disorders of behaviour. The British Journal of Psychiatry 157: 86–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burns, J.M., and R.H. Swerdlow. 2003. Right orbitofrontal tumor with pedophilia symptom and constructional apraxia sign. Archives of Neurology 60: 437–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cases, O., I. Seif, J. Grimsby, P. Gaspar, K. Chen, S. Pournin, U. Muller, M. Aguet, C. Babinet, J.C. Shih, and E. De Maeyer. 1995. Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science 268: 1763–1766.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caspi, A., J. McClay, T.E. Moffitt, J. Mill, J. Martin, I.W. Craig, A. Taylor, and R. Poulton. 2002. Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science 297: 851–854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunnington, R., C. Windischberger, L. Deecke, and E. Moser. 2002. The preparation and execution of self-initiated and externally-triggered movement: a study of event-related fMRI. NeuroImage 15: 373–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, H., T. Grabowski, R. Frank, A.M. Galaburda, and A.R. Damasio. 1994. The return of Phineas Gage: clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science 264: 1102–1105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farwell, L., and S.S. Smith. 2001. Using brain MERMER testing to detect knowledge despite efforts to conceal. Journal of Forensic Sciences 46: 135–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frazzetto, G., G. Di Lorenzo, V. Carola, L. Proietti, E. Sokolowska, A. Siracusano, C. Gross, and A. Troisi. 2007. Early trauma and increased risk for physical aggression during adulthood: the moderating role of MAOA genotype. PLoS ONE 2: e486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fried, I., R. Mukamel, and G. Kreiman. 2011. Internally generated preactivation of single neurons in human medial frontal cortex predicts volition. Neuron 69: 548–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, S.J., and P.W. Burgess. 2008. Executive function. In Current biology, R110–R114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gräff, J., and I.M. Mansuy. 2008. Epigenetic codes in cognition and behaviour. Behavioural Brain Research 192: 70–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haggard, P. 2008. Human volition: toward a neuroscience of will. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9: 934–946.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haggard, P. 2011. Decision time for free will. Neuron 69: 404–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haynes, J.-D., K. Sakai, G. Rees, S. Gilbert, C. Frith, and R.E. Passingham. 2007. Reading hidden intentions in the human brain. Current Biology 17: 323–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hou, L., D. Wang, and A. Baccarelli. 2011. Environmental chemicals and microRNAs. Mutation Research 714: 105–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, I.H., M. Jahanshani, M. Jueptner, R.E. Passingham, and D.J. Brooks. 2000. Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. Brain 123: 1216–1228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, P., K. Fenger, T.G. Bolwig, and S.A. Sørensen. 1998. Crime in Huntington’s disease: a study of registered offences among patients, relatives and controls. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 65: 467–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kandel, E.R., J.H. Schwartz, and T.M. Jessell. 2000. Principles of neural science, 4th ed. New York, USA: McGraw-Hill Companies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim-Cohen, J., A. Caspi, A. Taylor, B. Williams, R. Newcombe, I.W. Craig, and T.E. Moffitt. 2006. MAOA, maltreatment, and gene-environment interaction predicting children’s mental health: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry 11: 903–913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, H.C., R.D. Rogers, P. Haggard, and R.E. Passingham. 2004. Attention to intention. Science 303: 1208–1210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levenson, J.M., and J.D. Sweatt. 2005. Epigenetic mechanisms in memory formation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6: 108–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B. 1985. Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8: 529–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B. 2011. Do we have free will? In Conscious will and responsibility, ed. W. Sinnott-Armstrong, and L. Nadel, 1–10. New York, USA: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B., E.W. Wright Jr, and C.A. Gleason. 1982. Readiness-potentials preceding unrestricted ‘spontaneous’ vs. Pre-planned voluntary acts. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 54: 322–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B., C.A. Gleason, E.W. Wright, and D.K. Pearl. 1983. Time of conscious intent to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). Brain 106: 623–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mele, A. 2011. Libet on free will: readiness potentials, decisions, and awareness. In Conscious will and responsibility, ed. W. Sinnott-Armstrong, and L. Nadel, 23–33. New York, USA: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeith, I., and J. Cummings. 2005. Behavioural changes and psychological symptoms in dementia disorders. Lancet Neurology 4: 735–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E.K., and J.D. Cohen. 2001. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience 24: 167–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mobbs, D., H.C. Lau, O.D. Jones, and C.D. Frith. 2007. Law, responsibility, and the brain. PLoS Biology 5: e103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T.E. 2005. The new look of behavioral genetics in developmental psychopathology: gene-environment interplay in antisocial behaviors. Psychological Bulletin 131: 533–554.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, K.V., and J.S. Mattick. 2014. The rise of regulatory RNA. Nature Reviews Genetics 15: 423–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muehllehner, G., and J.S. Karp. 2006. Positron emission tomography. Physics in Medicine and Biology 51: R117–R137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nachev, P., C. Kennard, and M. Husain. 2008. Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9: 856–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, D. 2006. The music of life. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuffield. 2005. The ethics of research involving animals. London, UK: Nuffield Council on Bioethics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ollinger, J.M., and J.A. Fessler. 1997. Positron-emission tomography. IEEE Signal Processing Magazine, 43–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pockett, S., and S.C. Purdy. 2011. Are voluntary movements initiated preconsciously? The relationships between readiness potentials, urges and decisions. In Conscious will and responsibility, ed. W. Sinnott-Armstrong, and L. Nadel, 34–46. New York, USA: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quereshi, I.A., and M.F. Mehler. 2012. Emerging roles of non-coding RNAs in brain evolution, development, plasticity and disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13: 528–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raine, A. 2008. From genes to brain to antisocial behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science 17: 323–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rang, H.P., M.M. Dale, J.M. Ritter, and R.J. Flower. 2007. Rang and Dale’s pharmacology, 6th ed. Edinburgh, UK: Churchill Livingstone.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhee, S.H., and I.D. Waldman. 2002. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin 128: 490–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., L. Fogassi, and V. Gallese. 2001. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2: 661–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roskies, A. 2006. Neuroscientific challenges to free will and responsibility. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10: 419–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roskies, A.L. 2011. Why Libet’s studies don’t pose a threat to free will. In Conscious will and responsibility, ed. W. Sinnott-Armstrong, and L. Nadel, 11–22. New York, USA: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salat, D.H., J.A. Kaye, and J.S. Janowsky. 2002. Greater orbital prefrontal volume selectively predicts worse working memory performance in older adults. Cerebral Cortex 12: 494–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shumay, E., J. Logan, Volkow, N.D., and J.S. Fowler. 2012. Evidence that the methylation state of the monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene predicts brain activity of MAOA enzyme in healthy men. Epigenetics, 7: 1151–1160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, G., R. Saba, and G. Schratt. 2011. microRNAs in neurons: manifold regulatory roles at the synapse. Current Opinion in Genetics and Development 21: 491–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soon, C.S., M. Brass, H.-J. Heinze, and J.D. Haynes. 2008. Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience 11: 543–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spriggs, K.A., M. Bushell, and A.E. Willis. 2010. Translational regulation of gene expression during conditions of cell stress. Molecular Cell 40: 228–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Horn, J.D., A. Irimia, C.M. Torgerson, M.C. Chambers, R. Kikinis, and A.W. Toga. 2012. Mapping connectivity damage in the case of Phineas Gage. PLoS ONE 7: e37454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J.D., and F.H.C. Crick. 1953. Molecular structure of nucleic acids: a structure for deoxyribose nucleic acid. Nature 171: 737–738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, L.S., W. Davies, and A.R. Isles. 2007. Genomic imprinting effects on brain development and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8: 832–843.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yang, Y., and A. Raine. 2009. Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 174: 81–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chris Willmott .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Willmott, C. (2016). Biological Basis of Behaviour. In: Biological Determinism, Free Will and Moral Responsibility. SpringerBriefs in Ethics. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-30391-8_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics