Advertisement

An Evolutionary Perspective, Sociophysiology, and Heritability

  • Mohammadreza Hojat
Chapter
  • 2k Downloads

Abstract

  • For a better grasp of empathy, we need to understand its evolutionary roots and its sociophysiological functions.

  • During the course of evolution, human beings have been endowed with an innate capacity to express and understand emotions from nonverbal cues, which contributed to survival and is conceptualized as primitive empathy.

  • Evidence showing infant’s reactive crying, facial mimicry, physiological synchronicity in interpersonal interactions, and universal expression of emotions indicates that aspects of interpersonal behaviors cannot be attributed to social learning.

  • Twin studies suggest that heritability is also a significant component of empathy more often when a measure of emotional empathy (rather than cognitive empathy) is used.

Keywords

Evolutionary perspective Verbal communication Nonverbal communication Sociophysiology Heritability Physiological synchronization Twin studies Monozygotic twins Dizygotic twins Facial mimicry Imitation Body posture 

References

  1. Adler, H. M. (2002). The sociophysiology of caring in the doctor-patient relationship. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 17, 874–882.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1994). Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expression following bilateral damage to human amygdala. Nature, 372, 669–672.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Allport, F. H. (1924). Social psychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  4. Ax, A. F. (1964). Goals and methods of psychophysiology. Psychophysiology, 1, 8–25.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Baldwin, D. A., & Baird, J. A. (2001). Discerning intentions in dynamic human action. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 171–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Basch, M. F. (1983). Empathic understanding: A review of the concept and some theoretical considerations. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 31, 101–126.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J. (1986). “I show how you feel”: Motor mimicry as a communicative act. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 322–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bayes, M. A. (1972). Behavioral cues of interpersonal warmth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 39, 333–339.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Berkowitz, L. (1984). Some effects of thoughts on anti and prosocial influences of media events: A cognitive-neoassociation analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 410-427. Boyd, R. W., & DiMascio, A. (1957). Social behavior and automatic physiology: A sociophysiological study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 120, 207–212.Google Scholar
  10. Brothers, L. (1989). A biological perspective on empathy. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 10–19.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Buck, R., & Ginsburg, B. (1997a). Communicative genes and the evolution of empathy: Selfish and social emotions as voices of selfish and social genes. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 807, 481–483.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Buck, R., & Ginsburg, B. (1997b). Communicative genes and the evolution of empathy. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 17–43). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  13. Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differences: Origins through sexual selection. American Psychologist, 50, 164–168.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Buss, D. M. (2003). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  15. Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–232.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M. C., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 5497–5502. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0935845100.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of emotion in man and animals. New York: St. Martin’s Press (Originally published in 1872).Google Scholar
  19. Darwin, C. (1981). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. NJ: Princeton University Press (Originally published in 1871).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Davis, M. R. (1985). Perception of affective reverberation component. In A. P. Goldstein & G. Y. Michaels (Eds.), Empathy: Development, training, and consequences (pp. 62–108). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  22. Davis, M. H., Luce, C., & Kraus, S. J. (1994). The heritability of characteristics associated with dispositional empathy. Journal of Personality, 62, 369–391.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Dawkins, R. (1999). The selfish gene. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Dawson, G. (1994). Development of emotional expression and emotion regulation in infancy: Contribution of the frontal lobe. In G. Dawson & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain (pp. 346–379). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  25. de Quervain, D. J. F., Fischbacher, U., Treyer, V., Schellhammer, M., Schnyder, U., Buck, A., & Fehr, E. (2004). The neutral basis of altruistic punishment. Science, 305, 1254–1258.Google Scholar
  26. Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavior and Cognitive Neuroscience Review, 3, 71–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2006). A social-neuroscience perspective on empathy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 54–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. DiMascio, A., Boyd, R. W., & Greenblatt, M. (1957). Physiological correlates of tension and antagonism during psychotherapy: A study of “interpersonal physiology.”. Psychosomatic Medicine, 19, 99–104.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K. (2000). Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 11, 86–89.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Eisenberg, N. (1989). Empathy and related emotional responses (Vol. 44). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  31. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1974). Detecting deception from the body or face. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 288–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 218, 179–181.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Goldstein, A. P., & Michaels, G. Y. (1985). Empathy: Development, training, and consequences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  35. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetic evolution of social behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Hess, U., Blairy, S., & Phillippot, P. (1999). Facial mimicry. In P. Phillippot, R. Feldman, & E. Coats (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 213–241). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Hoffman, M. L. (1978). Psychological and biological perspectives on altruism. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1, 323–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Hogan, R. (1969). Development of an empathy scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 307–316.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Humphrey, N. (1983). Consciousness regained. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  40. James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Jung, C. G. (1964). Man and his symbols. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  42. Kaplan, N. B., & Bloom, S. W. (1960). The use of sociological and social psychological concepts in physiological research. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 131, 128–134.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Knafo, A., Zahn-Waxler, C., Van Hule, C., Robinson, J. L., & Rhee, S. H. (2008). The developmental origins of disposition toward empathy: Genetic and environmental contributions. Emotion, 8, 737–752. Retrieved from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014179.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Kugiumutzakis, G. (1998). Neonatal imitation in the intersubjective companion space. In S. Braten (Ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny (pp. 63–88). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  45. La France, M. (1979). Nonverbal synchrony and rapport: Analysis by the cross-lag panel technique. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42, 66–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. La France, M. (1982). Posturre mirroring and rapport. In M. Davis (Ed.), Interaction rhythms: Periodicity in communication behavior (pp. 279–298). New York: Human Sciences Press.Google Scholar
  47. Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & Chartland, T. L. (2003). The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 145–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Lanzetta, J. T., & Englis, B. G. (1989). Expectation of cooperation and competition and their effects on observers’ vicarious emotional response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 543–554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Levenson, R. W., & Ruef, A. M. (1992). Empathy, a physiological substrate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 234–246.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Malno, R. B., Boag, T. J., & Smith, A. A. (1957). Physiological study of personal interaction. Psychosomatic Medicine, 19, 105–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Matthews, K. A., Batson, C. D., Horn, J., & Rosenman, R. H. (1981). Principles in his nature which interest him in the fortune of others: The heritability of empathic concern for others. Journal of Personality, 49, 237–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Maurer, R. E., & Tindall, J. H. (1983). Effect of postural congruence on client’s perception of counselor empathy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 30, 158–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Mayerson, E. W. (1976). Putting the ill at ease. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  54. Mehrabian, A. (1972). Nonverbal communication. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.Google Scholar
  55. Mehrabian, A., & Epstein, N. A. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality, 40, 525–543.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702–709.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Meltzoff, A. N., & Prinz, W. (2002). The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Mumford, L. (1967). The myth of the machine. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.Google Scholar
  60. Page, K. M., & Novak, M. A. (2002). Empathy leads to fairness. Bulletin of Mathematical Biology, 64, 1101–1116.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Plutchik, R. (1987). Evolutionary bases of empathy. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 38–46). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Preston, S. D., & deWaal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: It’s ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–20.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Reiser, M. F., Reeves, R. B., & Armington, J. (1955). Effects of variation in laboratory procedure and experimenter upon the ballistoeargram, blood pressure, and heart rate in healthy young men. Psychosomatic Medicine, 17, 185–199.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Ridley, M., & Dawkins, R. (1981). The natural selection of altruism. In J. P. Rushton & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior: Social, personality, and developmental perspectives (pp. 19–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  65. Rushton, J. P. (2004). Genetic and environmental contributions to pro-social attitudes: A twin study of social responsibility. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Biological Sciences, 271, 2583–2585. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Rushton, J. P., Fulker, D. W., Neale, M. C., Nias, D. K. B., & Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Altruism and aggression: The heritability of individual differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1192–1198.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in newborns. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Schaflen, A. E. (1964). The significance of posture in communication systems. Psychiatry, 27, 316–331.Google Scholar
  69. Scourfield, J., Martin, N., Lewis, G., & McGuffin, P. (1999). Heritability of social cognitive skills in children and adolescents. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Siegel, D. J. (1999). The developing mind: How relationships and the brain interact to shape who we are. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  71. Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn response to the cry of another infant. Developmental Psychology, 5, 136–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Szalita, A. B. (1976). Some thoughts on empathy. The eighteenth annual Frieda Fromm-Reichmann memorial lecture. Psychiatry, 39, 142–152.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man (pp. 136–179). Chicago, IL: Aldine.Google Scholar
  75. Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 1–13.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Westerman, M. A. (2005). What is interpersonal behavior? Post-cartesian approach to problematic interpersonal pattern and psychotherapy process. Review of General Psychology, 9, 16–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655–664.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Zahn-Waxler, C., Robinson, J. L., & Emde, R. N. (1992). The development of empathy in twins. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1038–1047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mohammadreza Hojat
    • 1
  1. 1.Sidney Kimmel Medical College Thomas Jefferson UniversityPhiladelphiaUSA

Personalised recommendations