Advertisement

Psychological Time

  • Dan ZakayEmail author
Part of the Studies in Brain and Mind book series (SIBM, volume 9)

Abstract

People live in a constantly changing dynamic environment. Our internal environment is also dynamic and is characterized by biological and mental processes that are in constant flux. Without these changes in both the external and internal environments, life on earth would not exist. The dimension along which all these changes occur is called “time.” Without dwelling on its exact nature, “time” can be represented by a clock and is a useful notion that provides a good explanation for physical phenomena in our external environment. Like other organisms, humans must be able to relate to “time” to survive and adjust to the external environment. This presupposes that information about “time” is conveyed and perceived. Since no known human perceptual system is dedicated to “time,” subjective temporal experiences are likely to compensate for this lack. Specifically, these subjective experiences may be based on internal changes in events as reflected by internal clocks or memory processes. These changes are monotonically correlated with “time” and thus can provide useful information about its passage. Psychological time is a subjective feeling which is related to the temporal experiences. Nevertheless, psychological time differs from “time,” because it is non-linear and because it is dependent on the nature of events occurring within a time period. The correspondence between psychological time and “time,” though imperfect, is enough to enable reality testing and normal cognitive and social functioning. This chapter discusses and analyzes psychological time, its functions and nature.

Keywords

Attentional gate Boredom Prospective time 

References

  1. Allan, Lorraine G. 1983. The perception of time. Perception & Psychophysics 26(5): 340–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aschoff, J. 1985. On the perception of time during prolonged temporal isolation. Human Neurobiology 4: 41–52.Google Scholar
  3. Augustine, Saint. 1955. Confessions. Trans. and ed. Albert C. Outler. http://www.ccel.org/ccel/Augustine/Confessions. Accessed 20 Aug 2014.
  4. Bentall, R.P. 1990. The illusion of reality: A review and integration of psychological research on hallucinations. Psychological Bulletin 107(1): 82–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Birx, H.J. (ed.). 2009. Encyclopedia of time. Los Angeles: Sage.Google Scholar
  6. Block, Richard A. 1989. Experiencing and remembering time: Affordances, context and cognition. In Time and human cognition: A life-span perspective, ed. Iris Levin and Dan Zakay, 333–361. Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Block, Richard A., and M.A. Reed. 1978. Remembered duration: Evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 4: 656–665.Google Scholar
  8. Block, Richard A., and Dan Zakay. 1997. Prospective and retrospective duration judgments. A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 4(2): 184–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Block, Richard A., E.J. George, and M.A. Reed. 1980. A watched pot sometimes boils: a study of duration experience. Acta Psychologica 46: 81–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Boltz, Marilyn G. 2005. Temporal dimensions of conversational interaction: The role of response latencies and pauses in social impression formation. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 24: 103–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bruce, Vicki, and Andy Young. 1986. Understanding face recognition. British Journal of Psychology 77(3): 305–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Buhusi, Catalin V., and Warren H. Meck. 2005. What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6: 755–765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Coren, S., L.M. Ward, and J.T. Enns. 1999. Sensation and perception, 5th ed. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  14. Damasio, Antonio R., T.J. Grabowski, A. Bechara, H. Damasio, L.L. Ponto, J. Parvizi, and R.D. Hichwa. 2000. Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nature Neuroscience 3(10): 1049–1057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Eagleman, David M. 2008. Human time perception and its illusions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 18: 131–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Flanagan, J.R., and A.M. Wing. 1997. The role of internal models in motion planning and control: Evidence from grip force adjustments during movements of hand-held loads. The Journal of Neuroscience 17(4): 1519–1528.Google Scholar
  17. Fraisse, P. 1963. The psychology of time. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  18. France, Louise. 2005. The death of yesterday. The Observer. January 23.Google Scholar
  19. Friedman, W.J. 2004. Time in autobiographical memory. Social Cognition 22(5): 591–605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gibbon, John, Russell M. Church, Stephen Fairhurst, and Alejandro Kacelnik. 1988. Scalar expectancy theory and choice between delayed rewards. Psychological Review 95(1): 102–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gibson, James J. 1975. Events are perceivable but time is not. In The study of time, vol. 2, ed. J.T. Fraser and N. Lawrence, 295–301. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gruber, R.P., and Richard A. Block. 2013. The flow of time as a perceptual illusion. The Journal of Mind and Behavior 34(1): 91–100.Google Scholar
  23. Hintzman, D.L., and Richard A. Block. 1971. Repetition and memory: Evidence for a multiple-trace hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology 88(3): 297–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hughes, Diane Owen, and Thomas R. Trautmann (eds.). 1995. Time: Histories and ethnologies. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
  25. James, William. 1890. The principles of psychology, vol. 1. New York: Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kahneman, Daniel. 1973. Attention and effort. New York: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  27. Kreitler, Shulamith, and Hans Kreitler. 1968. Dimensions of meaning and their measurement. Psychological Reports 23: 1307–1329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kreitler, Hans, and Shulamith Kreitler. 1972. The model of cognitive orientation: Towards a model of human behavior. British Journal of Psychology 63(1): 9–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Laje, Rodrigo, and Dean V. Buonomano. 2013. Robust timing and motor patterns by taming chaos in recurrent neural networks. Nature Neuroscience 16: 925–933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lavie, Peretz, and Wilse B. Webb. 1975. Time estimation in a long-term time-free environment. American Journal of Psychology 88: 177–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lejeune, Helga. 1998. Switching or gating? The attentional challenge in cognitive models of psychological time. Behavioural Processes 44(2): 127–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Levin, Iris. 1989. Principles underlying time measurement: The development of children’s constraints on counting. In Time and human cognition: A life-span perspective, ed. Iris Levin and Dan Zakay, 145–181. Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Levin, Iris, and F. Wilkening. 1989. Measuring time via counting. In Time and human cognition: A life-span perspective, ed. Iris Levin and Dan Zakay, 119–143. Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Loftus, E.F., J.W. Schooler, S.M. Boone, and D. Kline. 1987. Time went by so slowly: Overestimation of event duration by males and females. Applied Cognitive Psychology 1: 3–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Merchant, Hugo, Deborah L. Harrington, and Warren H. Meck. 2013. Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Neuroscience 36: 313–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Michon, John A. 1972. Processing of temporal information and the cognitive theory of time experience. In The study of time, ed. Julius T. Fraser, F.C. Haber, and C.H. Müller, 242–258. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Michon, John A. 1990. Implicit and explicit representations of time. In Models of psychological time, ed. Richard A. Block, 37–54. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  38. Osuna, Edgar Elias. 1985. The psychological cost of waiting. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 29: 82–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Reeves, Adam, and George Sperling. 1986. Attention gating in short-term visual memory. Psychological Review 93(2): 180–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Thomas, Ewart A.C., and Wanda B. Weaver. 1975. Cognitive processing and time perception. Perception & Psychophysics 17: 363–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Wearden, John H. 1995. Feeling the heat: Body temperature and the rate of subjective time, revisited. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section B 48(2): 129–141.Google Scholar
  42. Wittmann, Marc. 2009. Psychology and time. In Encyclopedia of time, ed. J.H. Birx, 1057–1064. Los Angeles: Sage.Google Scholar
  43. Wittmann, Marc, and Martin P. Paulus. 2008. Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends in Cognitive Science 12(1): 7–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Zajonc, Robert B. 1980. Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist 35(2): 151–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Zakay, Dan. 1989. Subjective time and attentional resource allocation: An integrated model of time estimation. In Time and human cognition: A life-span perspective, ed. Iris Levin and Dan Zakay, 365–397. Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Zakay, Dan. 1992. On prospective time estimation, temporal relevance and temporal uncertainty. In Time, action and cognition, ed. F. Macar, V. Pouthas, and W.J. Friedman, 109–111. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Zakay, Dan. 2000. Gating or switching? Gating is a better model of prospective timing. Behavioural Processes 50(1): 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Zakay, Dan. 2009. Temporal illusions. In Encyclopedia of time, ed. J.H. Birx, 1270–1272. Los Angeles: Sage.Google Scholar
  49. Zakay, Dan. 2012. Experiencing time in daily life. British Psychologist 25(8): 578–582.Google Scholar
  50. Zakay, Dan. 2014. Psychological time as information: The case of boredom. Frontiers in Psychology, Perception Science 5: 917. 1–5.Google Scholar
  51. Zakay, Dan, and A. Barak. 1984. Meaning and career decision making. Journal of Vocational Behavior 24(1): 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Zakay, Dan, and Jonathan Bentwich. 1997. The tricks and traps of perceptual illusions. In The mythomanias: The nature of deception and self-deception, ed. Michael S. Myslobodsky, 73–104. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  53. Zakay, Dan, and Richard A. Block. 1995. An attentional-gate model of prospective time estimation. In Time and the dynamic control of behavior, ed. M. Richelle, V. De Keyser, G. d’Ydewalle, and A. Vandierendonck, 167–178. Liège: Université de Liège.Google Scholar
  54. Zakay, Dan, and Richard A. Block. 1997. Temporal cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science 6(1): 12–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Zakay, Dan, Arie Bibi, and Daniel Algom. 2014a. Garner interference and temporal information processing. Acta Psychologica 147: 143–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Zakay, Dan, Dida Fleisig, and Neta David. 2014b. Prospective timing during conversations. In Time and temporality in language and human experience, ed. Barbara Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk and Krzysztof Kosecki, 185–197. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of PsychologyThe Interdisciplinary Center HerzliyaHerzliyaIsrael

Personalised recommendations