Advertisement

From the Intrinsic Motive Pulse of Infant Actions to the Life Time of Cultural Meanings

  • Colwyn TrevarthenEmail author
Part of the Studies in Brain and Mind book series (SIBM, volume 9)

Abstract

Research on the timing of spontaneous actions made from birth—for self-sensing of the infant’s own body posture and movements, for perceptual apprehension of objects or events in the outside world, and for intimate communication with a parent—proves that innate measures of time regulate prospective control of body actions of a coherent human Self. Infants are born capable of synchronising with, imitating and complementing expressions of a parent’s feelings and interests in proto-conversational exchanges. Affective appraisals of imagined consequences for the well-being of body and mind are signalled by expressive movements of emotion that are sympathetically shared. Within a few weeks infants join with rhythms of play in narratives of expressive display, learning how to participate in conventional rituals that tell stories of a “proto-habitus.” Before the end of the first year the child shares conventional use of objects and develops proto-linguistic symbolic coding of actions that identify objects to be shared in cooperation with familiar companions.

Infants’ movements have universal temporal parameters of “musicality,” as cultivated in the “imitative arts” of theatre, dance, song and music. The same measures of “pulse,” “quality” and “narrative” are also evident in serial ordering of steps in completion of practical projects and in propositional representations. This primary sensori-motor intelligence and its intersubjective “vitality dynamics” must be considered as fundamental in any philosophy of the nature of our experience of time and its cultivated uses and measures. Understanding of inner “life time” benefits from a neuro-psychology of movements and their prospective perceptual control, a science developed over the past century.

Keywords

Movement times Vitality dynamics Affects Intentions Proto-conversation Play Music Imitation Education Non-verbal therapies Language origins 

References

  1. Abram, David. 1996. The spell of the sensuous: Perception and language in a more-than-human world. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
  2. Ammaniti, Massimo, and Vittorio Gallese. 2014. The birth of intersubjectivity: Psychodynamics, neurobiology, and the self. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  3. Athey, Chris. 1990. Extending thought in young children: A parent-teacher partnership. London: Paul Chapman Publishing.Google Scholar
  4. Bateson, Mary C. 1979. The epigenesis of conversational interaction: A personal account of research development. In Before speech: The beginning of human communication, ed. M. Bullowa, 63–77. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Beebe, B., J. Jaffe, S. Feldstein, K. Mays, and D. Alson. 1985. Inter-personal timing: The application of an adult dialogue model to mother-infant vocal and kinesic interactions. In Social perception in infants, ed. F.M. Field and N. Fox, 217–248. Norwood: Ablex.Google Scholar
  6. Bernstein, N. 1967. Coordination and regulation of movements. New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  7. Birdwhistell, R. 1970. Kinesics and context. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
  8. Bjørkvold, Jon-Roar. 1992. The muse within: Creativity and communication, song and play from childhood through maturity. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
  9. Bråten, Stein. 1988. Dialogic mind: The infant and adult in protoconversation. In Nature, cognition and system, ed. M. Cavallo, 187–205. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bråten, Stein. 1998. Infant learning by alterocentric participation: The reverse of egocentric observation in autism. In Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, ed. S. Bråten, 105–124. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Bråten, Stein. 2009. The intersubjective mirror in infant learning and evolution of speech. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Brazelton, T. Berry. 1979. Evidence of communication during neonatal behavioural assessment. In Before speech: The beginning of human communication, ed. M. Bullowa, 79–88. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Brazelton, T. Berry, and Joshua D. Sparrow. 2006. Touchpoints 0–3: Your child’s emotional and behavioral development, vol. I. Cambridge, MA: DaCapo Press.Google Scholar
  14. Bruce, Tina (ed.). 2012. Early childhood practice: Froebel today. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  15. Bruner, Jerome S. 1990. Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Bruner, Jerome S. 1996. The culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Bullowa, M. 1979a. Introduction. Prelinguistic communication: A field for scientific research. In Before speech: The beginning of human communication, ed. M. Bullowa, 1–62. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Bullowa, Margaret (ed.). 1979b. Before speech: The beginning of human communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Buzsáki, György. 2006. Rhythms of the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Castiello, Umberto, Cristina Becchio, Stefania Zoia, Cristian Nelini, Luisa Sartori, Laura Blason, Giuseppina D’Ottavio, Maria Bulgheroni, and Vittorio Gallese. 2010. Wired to be social: The ontogeny of human interaction. PLoS ONE 5(10): 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Clynes, Manfred. 1978. The touch of the emotions. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  22. Clynes, Manfred (ed.). 1982. Music, mind, and brain. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  23. Condon, William S., and William D. Ogston. 1967. A segmentation of behavior. Journal of Psychiatric Research 5: 221–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Condon, William S., and Louis S. Sander. 1974. Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: Interactional participation and language acquisition. Science 183: 99–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Craig, Cathy M., and David N. Lee. 1999. Neonatal control of nutritive sucking pressure: Evidence for an intrinsic tau-guide. Experimental Brain Research 124: 371–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Damasio, Antonio R. 2010. Self comes to mind: Constructing the conscious brain. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
  27. Darwin, Charles. 1859. On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
  28. Darwin, Charles. 1872/1998. The expression of the emotions in man and animals, 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  29. de Vries, J.I.P., Gerard H.A. Visser, and Heinz F.R. Prechtl. 1982. The emergence of fetal behavior, I: Qualitative aspects. Early Human Development 7: 301–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. DeCasper, Anthony J., and Phyllis Prescott. 2009. Lateralized processes constrain auditory reinforcement in human newborns. Hearing Research 255: 135–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Delafield-Butt, Jonathan, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 2013. Theories of the development of human communication. In Theories and models of communication: Handbook of communication science, vol. 1, ed. P. Cobley and P.J. Schultz, 199–221. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
  32. Delafield-Butt, Jonathan, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 2015. The origins of narrative. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1157. doi: 10.3389/psyg.2015.01157.
  33. Delamont, R. Shane, Peter O.O. Julu, and Goran A. Jamal. 1999. Periodicity of a noninvasive measure of cardiac vagal tone during non-rapid eye movement sleep in non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived normal subjects. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 16(2): 146–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Dewey, John. 1938. Experience and education. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  35. Dissanayake, Ellen. 2000. Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother-infant interaction. In The origins of music, ed. N.L. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown, 389–410. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  36. Dissanayake, E. 2009. Root, leaf, blossom or burl: Concerning the origin and adaptive function of music. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 17–30. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Donald, Merlin. 2001. A mind so rare: The evolution of human consciousness. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  38. Donaldson, Margaret. 1978. Children’s minds. Glasgow: Fontana/Collins.Google Scholar
  39. Donaldson, Margaret. 1992. Human minds: An exploration. London: Allen Lane/Penguin Books.Google Scholar
  40. Eckerdal, Patricia, and Bjorn Merker. 2009. “Music” and the “action song” in infant development: An interpretation. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 241–262. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Ekman, Paul, and Wallace V. Friesen. 1969. The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica 22: 353–374.Google Scholar
  42. Erickson, Fred. 2009. Musicality in talk and listening: A key element in classroom discourse as an environment for learning. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 449–464. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Feldman, Ruth. 2012. Parent-infant synchrony: A bio-behavioral model of mutual influences in the formation of affiliative bonds. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 77(2): 42–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Fraisse, Paul. 1982. Rhythm and tempo. In The psychology of music, ed. D. Deutsch, 149–180. New York: Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Frank, Bodo, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 2012. Intuitive meaning: Supporting impulses for interpersonal life in the sociosphere of human knowledge, practice and language. In Moving ourselves, moving others: Motion and emotion in intersubjectivity, consciousness and language, ed. A. Foolen, U.M. Lüdtke, T.P. Racine, and J. Zlatev, 261–303. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Frijda, Nico H. 2007. The laws of emotion. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  47. Gibson, James J. 1979. The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  48. Glickstein, Mitchell, and Karl Doron. 2008. Cerebellum: Connections and functions. Cerebellum 7: 589–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Goodrich, Barbara G. 2010. We do, therefore we think: Time, motility, and consciousness. Reviews in the Neurosciences 21(5): 331–361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Gratier, Maya, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 2007. Voice, vitality and meaning: On the shaping of the infant’s utterances in willing engagement with culture. International Journal for Dialogical Science 2(1): 169–181.Google Scholar
  51. Gratier, Maya, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 2008. Musical narrative and motives for culture in mother-infant vocal interaction. Journal of Consciousness Studies 15(10–11): 122–158.Google Scholar
  52. Hadamard, Jacques. 1945. The psychology of invention in the mathematical field. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  53. Halliday, Michael A.K. 1978. Meaning and the construction of reality in early childhood. In Modes of perceiving and processing information, ed. H.L. Pick and F. Saltzman, 67–96. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  54. Halliday, Michael A.K. 1979. One child’s protolanguage. In Before speech: The beginning of human communication, ed. M. Bullowa, 171–190. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Halliday, Michael A.K., and Christian M.I.M. Matthiessen. 2004. An introduction to functional grammar, 3rd ed. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
  56. Helmholtz, Hermann von. 1867. Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik. Leipzig: Leopold Voss.Google Scholar
  57. Hess, Walter Rudolf. 1954. Diencephalon: Autonomic and extrapyramidal functions. Orlando: Grune and Stratton.Google Scholar
  58. Hevner, Robert F. 2000. Development of connections in the human visual system during fetal midgestation: A Dii-tracing study. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology 59: 385–392.Google Scholar
  59. Hubley, Penelope, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 1979. Sharing a task in infancy. In Social interaction during infancy: New directions for child development, vol. 4, ed. I. Uzgiris, 57–80. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  60. Husserl, Edmund. 1964. The phenomenology of internal time consciousness. Trans. J. Churchill. Bloomingtin: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  61. Jakobovits, Akos A. 2009. Grasping activity in utero: A significant indicator of fetal behavior. Journal of Perinatal Medicine 37: 571–572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. James, William. 1890. The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. James, David, Mary Pillai, and John Smoleniec. 1995. Neurobehavioral development of the human fetus. In Fetal development: A psychobiological perspective, ed. J.-P. Lecanuet, W.P. Fifer, N.A. Krasnegor, and W.P. Smotherman, 101–128. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  64. Jeannerod, Marc. 1994. The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17: 187–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Kendon, Adam. 1980. Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In The relationship of verbal and nonverbal communication, ed. M.R. Key, 207–227. The Hague: Mouton and Co.Google Scholar
  66. Kugiumutzakis, Giannis, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 2015. Neonatal imitation. Elsevier international encyclopedia of social and behavioural sciences, Area 2, Entry 23160, In press.Google Scholar
  67. Kühl, Ole. 2007. Musical semantics. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
  68. Langer, Susanne K. 1942. Philosophy in a new key: A study in the symbolism of reason, rite, and art. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  69. Langer, Susanne K. 1953. Feeling and form. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  70. Larroche, Jeanne-Claudie. 1981. The marginal layer in the neocortex of a 7 week-old human embryo: A light and electron microscopy study. Anatomy and Embryology 162: 301–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Lashley, Karl S. 1951. The problems of serial order in behavior. In Cerebral mechanisms in behavior, ed. L.A. Jeffress, 112–136. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  72. Lecanuet, Jean-Pierre, William P. Fifer, Norman A. Krasnegor, and William P. Smotherman (eds.). 1995. Fetal development: A psychobiological perspective. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  73. Lee, David N. 2009. General tau theory: Evolution to date. Perception 38: 837–858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Lee, David N., and Benjamin Schögler. 2009. Tau in musical expression. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 83–104. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  75. Lee, David N., C.M. Craig, and M.A. Grealy. 1999. Sensory and intrinsic coordination of movement. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B 266: 2029–2035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Lenneberg, Eric. 1967. Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  77. Llinás, Rodolfo. 2001. I of the vortex: From neurons to self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  78. MacLean, Paul D. 1990. The triune brain in evolution, role in paleocerebral functions. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  79. Malloch, Stephen. 1999. Mother and infants and communicative musicality. In Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 1999–2000, ed. I. Deliège, 29–57. Liège: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music.Google Scholar
  80. Malloch, Stephen, and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds.). 2009a. Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  81. Malloch, Stephen, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 2009b. Musicality: Communicating the vitality and interests of life. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 1–11. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  82. Maturana, Humberto R., and Francisco J. Varela. 1980. Autopoiesis and cognition. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Maturana, Humberto, J. Mpodozis, and J. Carlos Letelier. 1995. Brain, language and the origin of human mental functions. Biological Research 28(1): 15–26.Google Scholar
  84. Mead, George Herbert. 1934. Mind, self, and society from the standpoint of a social behaviorist. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  85. Meares, Russell. 2004. The conversational model: An outline. American Journal of Psychotherapy 58: 51–66.Google Scholar
  86. Meares, Russell, Nick Bendit, Joan Haliburn, Anthony Korner, Dawn Mears, and David Butt. 2012. Borderline personality disorder and the conversational model: A clinician’s manual. New York: W.W. Norton and Company.Google Scholar
  87. Melges, Frederick T. 1982. Time and the inner future: A temporal approach to psychiatric disorders. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  88. Merker, Bjorn. 2005. The liabilities of mobility: A selection pressure for the transition to consciousness in animal evolution. Consciousness and Cognition 14(1): 89–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Merker, Bjorn. 2007. Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30: 63–134.Google Scholar
  90. Merker, Bjorn. 2009a. Ritual foundations of human uniqueness. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 45–60. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  91. Merker, Bjorn. 2009b. Returning language to culture by way of biology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32(5): 460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Merleau-Ponty, Maurice. 1962. Phenomenology of perception. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  93. Miall, David S., and Ellen Dissanayake. 2003. The poetics of babytalk. Human Nature 14(4): 337–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Mo, Suchoon S. 1990. Time reversal in human cognition: Search for a temporal theory of insanity. In Cognitive models of psychological time, ed. R.A. Block, 241–254. Hilldale: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  95. Moruzzi, Giuseppe, and Horace W. Magoun. 1949. Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1: 455–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Nadel, Jacqueline. 2014. How imitation boosts development: In typical infants and children with autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  97. Nadel, Jacqueline, and Ann Pezé. 1993. Immediate imitation as a basis for primary communication in toddlers and autistic children. In New perspectives in early communicative development, ed. J. Nadel and L. Camioni, 139–156. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  98. Nagy, Emese. 2006. From imitation to conversation: The first dialogues with human neonates. Infant and Child Development 15: 223–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Nagy, Emese. 2011. The newborn infant: A missing stage in developmental psychology. Infant and Child Development 20(1): 3–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Nagy, Emese, and Peter Molnár. 2004. Homo imitans or homo provocans? The phenomenon of neonatal imitiation. Infant Behavior and Development 27: 57–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Okado, Nobuo. 1980. Development of the human cervical spinal cord with reference to synapse formation in the motor nucleus. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 191: 495–513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Osborne, Nigel. 2009a. Towards a chronobiology of musical rhythm. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 545–564. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  103. Osborne, Nigel. 2009b. Music for children in zones of conflict and post-conflict: A psychobiological approach. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 331–356. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  104. Panksepp, Jaak. 2007a. Emotional feelings originate below the neocortex: Toward a neurobiology of the soul. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30: 101–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Panksepp, Jaak. 2007b. Can PLAY diminish ADHD and facilitate the construction of the social brain? Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 16(2): 5–14.Google Scholar
  106. Panksepp, Jaak, and Jeffrey Burgdorf. 2003. “Laughing” rats and the evolutionary antecedents of human joy? Physiology and Behavior 79: 533–547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Panksepp, Jaak, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 2009. The neuroscience of emotion in music. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 105–146. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  108. Papoušek, Hanuš. 1967. Experimental studies of appetitional behaviour in human newborns and infants. In Early behaviour: Comparative and developmental approaches, ed. H.W. Stevenson, E.H. Hess, and H.L. Rheingold, 249–277. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  109. Papoušek, Hanuš. 1996a. Musicality in infancy research: Biological and cultural origins of early musicality. In Musical beginnings: Origins and development of musical competence, ed. I. Deliège and J. Sloboda, 37–55. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Papoušek, Mechthild. 1996b. Intuitive parenting: A hidden source of musical stimulation in infancy. In Musical beginnings: Origins and development of musical competence, ed. I. Deliège and J. Sloboda, 88–112. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Parncutt, Richard. 2006. Prenatal development. In The child as musician, ed. G.E. McPherson, 1–31. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Pavlicevic, Mercedes. 1997. Music therapy in context: Music meaning and relationship. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  113. Pavlicevic, Mercedes, and Gary Ansdell. 2009. Between communicative musicality and collaborative musicing: A perspective from community music therapy. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 357–376. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  114. Pavlicevic, Mercedes, Colwyn Trevarthen, and Janice Duncan. 1994. Improvisational music therapy and the rehabilitation of persons suffering from chronic schizophrenia. Journal of Music Therapy 31(2): 86–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Piontelli, Alessandra. 1992. From fetus to child. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  116. Piontelli, Alessandra. 2002. Twins: From fetus to child. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  117. Porges, Stephen W. 2001. The polyvagal theory: Phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. International Journal of Psychophysiology 42: 123–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. Porges, Stephen W. 2003. Social engagement and attachment: A phylogenetic perspective. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1008: 31–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Porges, Stephen W., and Senta A. Furman. 2011. The early development of the autonomic nervous system provides a neural platform for social behaviour: A polyvagal perspective. Infant and Child Development 20: 106–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Prechtl, Heinz F.R. 1984. Continuity and change in early human development. In Continuity of neural functions from prenatal to postnatal life, ed. H.F.R. Prechtl, 1–13. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  121. Reddy, Vasudevi. 2008. How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  122. Reddy, Vasudevi. 2012. A gaze at grips with me. In Joint attention: New developments in psychology, philosophy of mind and social neuroscience, ed. A. Seemans, 137–157. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  123. Reid, Thomas. 1764/1997. An inquiry into the human mind on the principles of common sense, ed. Derek R. Brookes. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
  124. Reissland, Nadia, Brian Francis, James Mason, and Karen Lincoln. 2011. Do facial expressions develop before birth? PLoS ONE 6(8): 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Robarts, Jacqueline Z. 2009. Supporting the development of mindfulness and meaning: Clinical pathways in music therapy with a sexually abused child. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 377–400. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  126. Rodrıguez, Cintia, and Pedro Palacios. 2007. Do private gestures have a self-regulatory function? A case study. Infant Behavior and Development 30: 180–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  127. Rogoff, Barbara. 2003. The cultural nature of human development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  128. Royal College of Obstetricians and Gynaecologists. 2010. Fetal awareness: Review of research and recommendations for practice. London: Royal College of Obstetricians and Gynaecologists.Google Scholar
  129. Schefflen, Albert E. 1973. Communicational structure: Analysis of a psychotherapy transaction. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  130. Schögler, Benjaman, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 2007. To sing and dance together. In On being moved: From mirror neurons to empathy, ed. S. Bråten, 281–302. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Schore, Allan. 2012a. Bowlby’s “environment of evolutionary adaptiveness”: Recent studies on the interpersonal neurobiology of attachment and emotional development. In Evolution, early experience and human development: From research to practice and policy, ed. D. Narvaez, J. Panksepp, A. Schore, and T. Gleason, 31–67. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  132. Schore, Allan. 2012b. The science of the art of psychotherapy. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
  133. Sebeok, Thomas A. 1994. Signs: An introduction to semiotics. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
  134. Sherrington, Charles S. 1906. The integrative action of the nervous system. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  135. Smith, Adam. 1777/1982. Of the nature of that imitation which takes place in what are called the imitative arts. In Essays on philosophical subjects, ed. W.P.D. Wightman, and J.C. Bryce. Indianapolis: Liberty Fund.Google Scholar
  136. Solms, Marc, and Jaak Panksepp. 2012. The “Id” knows more than the “Ego” admits: Neuropsychoanalytic and primal consciousness perspectives on the interface between affective and cognitive neuroscience. Brain Sciences 2: 147–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  137. Sperry, Roger Wolcott. 1950. Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 43: 482–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  138. Sperry, Roger Wolcott. 1952. Neurology and the mind-brain problem. American Scientist 40: 291–312.Google Scholar
  139. Sperry, Roger Wolcott. 1983. Science and moral priority. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  140. Stern, Daniel N. 1974. Mother and infant at play: The dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal and gaze behaviours. In The effect of the infant on its caregiver, ed. M. Lewis and L.A. Rosenblum, 187–213. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  141. Stern, Daniel N. 2000. The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and development psychology, 2nd ed. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  142. Stern, Daniel N. 2004. The present moment in psychotherapy and everyday life. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
  143. Stern, Daniel N. 2010. Forms of vitality: Exploring dynamic experience in psychology, the arts, psychotherapy and development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  144. Stern, Daniel N., Kristine MacKain, and Susan Spieker. 1982. Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. Developmental Psychology 18: 727–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  145. Stern, Daniel N., Lynne Hofer, Wendy Haft, and John Dore. 1985. Affect attunement: The sharing of feeling states between mother and infant by means of inter-modal fluency. In Social perception in infants, ed. T.M. Field and N.A. Fox, 249–268. Norwood: Ablex.Google Scholar
  146. Stern, Daniel N., Louis W. Sander, Jeremy P. Nahum, Alexandra M. Harrison, Karlen Lyons-Ruth, Alexander C. Morgan, Nadia Bruschweiler-Stern, and Edward Z. Tronick. 1998. Non-interpretive mechanisms in psychoanalytic therapy: The “something more” than interpretation. International Journal of Psychoanalysis 79: 903–921.Google Scholar
  147. Stige, Brunholf. 2004. Community music therapy: Culture, care and welfare. In Community music therapy, ed. M. Pavlicevic and G. Ansdell, 91–113. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  148. Stuart, Susan A.J. 2010. Enkinaesthesia, biosemiotics and the ethiosphere. In Signifying bodies: Biosemiosis, interaction and health, ed. S. Cowley, J.C. Major, S. Steffensen, and A. Dini, 305–330. Braga: Portuguese Catholic University.Google Scholar
  149. Takada, Akira. 2005. Mother-infant interactions among the !Xun: Analysis of gymnastic and breastfeeding behaviors. In Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental, and cultural perspectives, ed. B.S. Hewlett and M.E. Lamb, 289–308. New Brunswick: Aldine Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
  150. Thelin, Nils. 2014. On the nature of time: A biopragmatic perspective on language, thought and reality. Uppsala: Uppsala University.Google Scholar
  151. Tomasello, Michael. 2008. Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  152. Trainor, Laurel J. 1996. Infant preferences for infant-directed versus non-infant-directed play songs and lullabies. Infant Behavior and Development 19: 83–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  153. Trehub, Sandra E. 1990. The perception of musical patterns by human infants: The provision of similar patterns by their parents. In Comparative perception, Vol. 1: Mechanisms, ed. M.A. Berkley and W.C. Stebbins, 429–459. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  154. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1974. The psychobiology of speech development. In Language and brain: Developmental aspects, ed. E.H. Lenneberg, 570–585. Boston: Neurosciences Research Program.Google Scholar
  155. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1979. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before speech: The beginning of human communication, ed. M. Bullowa, 321–347. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  156. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1984. How control of movements develops. In Human motor actions: Bernstein reassessed, ed. H.T.A. Whiting, 223–261. Amsterdam: Elsevier (North Holland).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  157. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1985. Neuroembryology and the development of perceptual mechanisms. In Human growth, 2nd ed, ed. F. Falkner and J.M. Tanner, 301–383. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  158. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1986. Development of intersubjective motor control in infants. In Motor development in children: Aspects of coordination and control, ed. M.G. Wade and H.T.A. Whiting, 209–261. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhof.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  159. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1990. Signs before speech. In The semiotic web, ed. T.A. Sebeok and J. Umiker-Sebeok, 689–755. Amsterdam: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
  160. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1992. An infant’s motives for speaking and thinking in the culture. In The dialogical alternative: Towards a theory of language and mind (Festschrift for Ragnar Rommetveit), ed. A.H. Wold, 99–137. Oslo/Oxford: Scandinavian University Press/Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  161. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1996. Lateral asymmetries in infancy: Implications for the development of the hemispheres. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 20(4): 571–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  162. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1999. Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. In Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 1999–2000, ed. I. Deliège, 157–213. Liège: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music.Google Scholar
  163. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2001. The neurobiology of early communication: Intersubjective regulations in human brain development. In Handbook on brain and behavior in human development, ed. A.F. Kalverboer and A. Gramsbergen, 841–882. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  164. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2004a. Language development: Mechanisms in the brain. In Encyclopedia of neuroscience, 3rd ed, ed. G. Adelman and B.H. Smith. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
  165. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2004b. Brain development. In Oxford companion to the mind, 2nd ed, ed. R.L. Gregory, 116–127. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  166. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2005a. First things first: Infants make good use of the sympathetic rhythm of imitation, without reason or language. Journal of Child Psychotherapy 31(1): 91–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  167. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2005b. Stepping away from the mirror: Pride and shame in adventures of companionship. Reflections on the nature and emotional needs of infant intersubjectivity. In Attachment and bonding: A new synthesis, ed. C.S. Carter, L. Ahnert, K.E. Grossmann, S.B. Hrdy, M.E. Lamb, S.W. Porges, and N. Sachser, 55–84. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  168. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2008. The musical art of infant conversation: Narrating in the time of sympathetic experience, without rational interpretation, before words, 11–37. Special Issue: Musicae Scientiae.Google Scholar
  169. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2009a. The functions of emotion in infancy: The regulation and communication of rhythm, sympathy, and meaning in human development. In The healing power of emotion: Affective neuroscience, development, and clinical practice, ed. D. Fosha, D.J. Siegel, and M.F. Solomon, 55–85. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
  170. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2009b. Human biochronology: On the source and functions of “musicality”. In Music that works: Contributions of biology, neurophysiology, psychology, sociology, medicine and musicology, ed. R. Haas and V. Brandes, 221–265. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  171. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2011. What young children give to their learning, making education work to sustain a community and its culture. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 19(2): 173–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  172. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2012a. Communicative musicality: The human impulse to create and share music. In Musical imaginations: Multidisciplinary perspectives on creativity, performance, and perception, ed. D.J. Hargreaves, D.E. Miell, and R.A.R. MacDonald, 259–284. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  173. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2012b. Born for art, and the joyful companionship of fiction. In Evolution, early experience and human development: From research to practice and policy, ed. D. Narvaez, J. Panksepp, A. Schore, and T. Gleason, 202–218. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  174. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2012c. Embodied human intersubjectivity: Imaginative agency, to share meaning. Cognitive Semiotics 4(1): 6–56.Google Scholar
  175. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2012d. The generation of human meaning: How shared experience grows in infancy. In Joint attention: New developments in philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience, ed. A. Seemann, 73–113. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  176. Trevarthen, Colwyn, and K.J. Aitken. 2001. Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, Annual Research Review 42(1): 3–48.Google Scholar
  177. Trevarthen, Colwyn, and K.J. Aitken. 2003. Regulation of brain development and age-related changes in infants’ motives: The developmental function of “regressive” periods. In Regression periods in human infancy, ed. M. Heimann, 107–184. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  178. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2015. Infant Semiosis: The psycho-biology of action and shared experience from birth. In Semiotics and cognition in human development, eds. Chris Sinha, Göran Sonesson and Sara Lenninger, Special Issue in Cognitive Development (in press).Google Scholar
  179. Trevarthen, Colwyn, and J. Delafield-Butt. 2013a. Biology of shared experience and language development: Regulations for the inter-subjective life of narratives. In The infant mind: Origins of the social brain, ed. M. Legerstee, D. Haley, and M. Bornstein, 167–199. New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
  180. Trevarthen, Colwyn, and J. Delafield-Butt. 2013b. Autism as a developmental disorder in intentional movement and affective engagement. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 7(49): 1–16.Google Scholar
  181. Trevarthen, Colwyn, and Penelope Hubley. 1978. Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In Action, gesture and symbol, ed. A. Lock, 183–229. London: Academic.Google Scholar
  182. Trevarthen, Colwyn, J. Delafield-Butt, and B. Schögler. 2011. Psychobiology of musical gesture: Innate rhythm, harmony and melody in movements of narration. In New perspectives on music and gesture, ed. A. Gritten and E. King, 11–43. Burlington: Ashgate.Google Scholar
  183. Trevarthen, Colwyn, M. Gratier, and N. Osborne. 2014. The human nature of culture and education. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 5: 173–192.Google Scholar
  184. Tulving, Endel. 2002. Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology 253: 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  185. Turner, Victor. 1982. From ritual to theatre: The human seriousness of play. New York: Performing Arts Journal Publications.Google Scholar
  186. Turner, Robert, and Andreas A. Ioannides. 2009. Brain, music and musicality: Inferences from neuroimaging. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 147–181. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  187. van Rees, Saskia, and Richard de Leeuw. 1993. Born too early: The kangaroo method with premature babies. Video by Stichting Lichaamstaal, Scheyvenhofweg 12, 6093 PR. Heythuysen.Google Scholar
  188. Vandekerckhove, Marie, Luis C. Bulnes, and Jaak Panksepp. 2014. The emergence of primary anoetic consciousness in episodic memory. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7(210): 1–8.Google Scholar
  189. von Holst, Erich. 1936. Versuche zur Theorie der relativen Koordination. Archives für Gesamte Physiologie 236: 93–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  190. von Holst, Erich, and Horst Mittelstaedt. 1950. Das Reafferenzprinzip. Naturwissenschaften 37: 256–272.Google Scholar
  191. von Uexküll, Jakob. 1957. A stroll through the worlds of animals and men: A picture book of invisible worlds. In Instinctive behavior. The development of a modern concept, ed. Claire H. Schiller, 5–80. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  192. Wallin, Nils L., Bjorn Merker, and Steven Brown (eds.). 2000. The origins of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  193. Whitehead, Alfred North. 1926. Science and the modern world. Lowell Lectures, 1925. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  194. Wigram, Tony, and Cochavit Elefant. 2009. Therapeutic dialogues in music: Nurturing musicality of communication in children with autistic spectrum disorder and Rett syndrome. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen, 423–445. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  195. Windle, William F. 1970. Development of neural elements in human embryos of four to seven weeks gestation. Experimental neurology (Suppl.) 5: 44–83.Google Scholar
  196. Zoia, Stefania, Laura Blason, Giuseppina D’Ottavio, Maria Bulgheroni, Eva Pezzetta, Aldo Scabar, and Umberto Castiello. 2007. Evidence of early development of action planning in the human foetus: A kinematic study. Experimental Brain Research 176: 217–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language SciencesThe University of EdinburghEdinburghUK

Personalised recommendations