Advertisement

Time in Intersubjectivity: Some Tools for Analysis

  • Bruno MölderEmail author
Part of the Studies in Brain and Mind book series (SIBM, volume 9)

Abstract

Proper timing and other temporal factors are often viewed as important for rhythmic and synchronised social interaction. The chapter attempts to clarify what roles temporal properties play in interpersonal coordination. The tools for investigating the role of time in intersubjectivity are taken from Craver’s account of explanation, which I extend to intersubjective processes. A distinction is made between causal relevance, constitutive relevance, temporal constraints and background conditions. With the help of these tools, various cases of intersubjective coordination are scrutinised: the interaction between mother and infant and Thomas Fuchs’ phenomenological accounts of schizophrenia and depression, where the disturbance is supposed to involve both intersubjectivity and temporality. Thanks to these fine-grained distinctions we can give specific and different verdicts about the role of time in each particular case.

Keywords

Intersubjectivity Subjective time Temporality Schizophrenia Depression Mechanism 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Work on this chapter was supported by the COST Action TD0904 (Time in Mental Activity), institutional research funding IUT20-5 of the Estonian Ministry of Education and Research and a grant ETF9117 from the Estonian Science Foundation. I am grateful to Colwyn Trevarthen for reading suggestions and to Alexander Davies for checking my English.

References

  1. Brazelton, T. Berry, Barbara Koslowski, and Mary Main. 1974. The origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant interaction. In The effect of the infant on its caregiver, ed. M. Lewis and L.A. Rosenblum, 49–76. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
  2. Craver, Carl. 2007. Explaining the brain: Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience. Oxford: Clarendon.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Dainton, Barry. 2006. Stream of consciousness: Unity and continuity in conscious experience, 2nd ed. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  4. De Jaegher, Hanne, Ezequiel Di Paolo, and Shaun Gallagher. 2010. Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(10): 441–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Droit-Volet, Sylvie, and Sandrine Gil. 2009. The time-emotion paradox. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society 364: 1943–1953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Droit-Volet, Sylvie, Sophie Fayolle, Mathilde Lamotte, and Sandrine Gil. 2013. Time, emotion and the embodiment of timing. Timing & Time Perception 1: 99–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fuchs, Thomas. 2001. Melancholia as a desynchronization: Towards a psychopathology of interpersonal time. Psychopathology 34: 179–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Fuchs, Thomas. 2005. Implicit and explicit temporality. Philosophy, Psychiatry & Psychology 8: 323–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fuchs, Thomas. 2007. The temporal structure of intentionality and its disturbance in schizophrenia. Psychopathology 40: 229–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fuchs, Thomas. 2013. Temporality and psychopathology. Phenomenology and Cognitive Sciences 12: 75–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gallagher, Shaun. 1998. The inordinance of time. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Gallagher, Shaun, and Daniel D. Hutto. 2008. Understanding others through primary interaction and narrative practice. In The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity, ed. J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, and E. Itkonen, 17–38. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gratier, Maya, and Gisèle Apter-Danon. 2009. The improvised musicality of belonging: Repetition and variation in mother-infant vocal interaction. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. Stephen Malloch and Colwyn Trevarthen, 301–327. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Halliday, Michael A.K. 1979. One child’s protolanguage. In Before speech: The beginning of human communication, ed. Margaret Bullowa, 171–190. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Husserl, Edmund. 1991. On the phenomenology of the consciousness of internal time (1893–1917). Trans. J. B. Brough. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
  16. Lloyd, Dan. 2012. Neural correlates of temporality: Default mode variability and temporal awareness. Consciousness and Cognition 21(2): 695–703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Malloch, Stephen, and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds.). 2009. Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Murray, Lynne, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 1985. Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Social perception in infants, ed. T.M. Field and N.A. Fox, 177–197. Norwood: Ablex.Google Scholar
  19. Nadel, Jacqueline, Isabelle Carchon, Claude Kervella, Daniel Marcelli, and Denis Réserbat-Plantey. 1999. Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science 2(2): 164–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Papoušek, Hanuš, and Mechthild Papoušek. 1995. Intuitive parenting. In Handbook of parenting: Biology and ecology of parenting, vol. 2, ed. Marc H. Bornstein, 117–136. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Piccinini, Gualtiero, and Carl Craver. 2011. Integrating psychology and neuroscience: Functional analyses as mechanism sketches. Synthese 183: 283–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ratcliffe, Matthew. 2012. Varieties of temporal experience in depression. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy 37: 114–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Rodemeyer, Lanei M. 2006. Intersubjective temporality: It’s about time. Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Ryan, Joanna. 1974. Early language development: Towards a communicational analysis. In The integration of a child into a social world, ed. M.P.M. Richards, 185–213. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  25. Spaulding, Shannon. 2012. Introduction to debates on embodied social cognition. Phenomenology and Cognitive Sciences 11: 431–448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1979. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before speech: The beginning of human communication, ed. Margaret Bullowa, 321–347. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1984. How control of movements develops. In Human motor actions: Bernstein reassessed, ed. H.T.A. Whiting, 223–261. Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1986. Development of intersubjective motor control in infants. In Motor development in children: Aspects of coordination and control, ed. M.G. Wade and H.T.A. Whiting, 209–261. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhof.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1999a. Intersubjectivity. In The MIT encyclopedia of cognitive sciences, ed. R.A. Wilson and F.C. Keil, 415–419. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  30. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1999b. Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae Scientiae 3(1): 157–213.Google Scholar
  31. Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2008. Shared minds and the science of fiction: Why theories will differ. In The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity, ed. J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, and E. Itkonen, vii–xii. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Trevarthen, Colwyn, and Penelope Hubley. 1978. Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language, ed. Andrew Lock, 183–229. London: Academic.Google Scholar
  33. Woodward, James. 2003. Making things happen. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Woodward, James, and Christopher Hitchcock. 2003. Explanatory generalizations, part I: A counterfactual account. Noûs 37(1): 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Zahavi, Dan. 2003. Husserl’s phenomenology. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Zahavi, Dan. 2005. Subjectivity and selfhood: Investigating the first-person perspective. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute of Philosophy and SemioticsUniversity of TartuTartuEstonia

Personalised recommendations