Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 11))

Abstract

The chapter addresses the topic of developing literacy from the perspective of “later language development”, focusing on linguistic literacy. Attitudes to written language as a key component of linguistic literacy are reviewed across different periods in history and from the perspective of different disciplines. Lexical, syntactic, and discursive features of text construction are then analyzed as relatively more or less impacted by whether the medium of expression is speech or writing. The data-base consists of 160 personal-experience narratives produced by the same participants in both speech and writing, half in Californian English, half in Israeli Hebrew, from four age/schooling levels (middle childhood, pre-adolescence, adolescence, and educated adults). The chapter concludes by considering what changes in developing linguistic literacy from grade-school to middle- and high-school as manifested in writing/speech distinctiveness. Modality-driven differences between the two means of expression are evident from the youngest age-group: Written texts show greater density in packaging of information, while their spoken counterparts are longer and include more repetitions, and disfluencies. That is, processing factors inherent in the output demands of each modality tend to apply irrespective of age. On the other hand, speech/writing distinctiveness manifests a somewhat U-shaped developmental curve: In 4th and 7th grade, written expression is still largely anchored in the more familiar medium of spoken language; later, from high-school on, increasing differentiation between writing and speech reflects the two modes of expression as distinct styles of discourse; at a third phase of knowledge integration, literate adults manifest bi-directional effects between the two modes of expression, such that their spoken language demonstrates the impact of familiarity with written discourse.

Parts of this chapter are revised and rewritten from a paper in Hebrew to appear in the Israel Journal of Literacy and Language. Thanks to Enav Kedar for help with style and editing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Concern here is with between-genre distinctions, so disregarding for present purposes related notions of different text-internal rhetorical functions (Giora 1990; Paltridge 2002) or “modes of discourse” (Du Bois 1980; Smith 2003) such as argumentation, classification, definition, description, or evaluation.

  2. 2.

    Dr. Bracha Nir was actively involved in developing all the relevant measures, as well as being responsible for statistical analyses, details of which are provided in a chapter written in Hebrew (Berman and Nir 2011). Graphs for each finding noted here can be supplied by the author on request.

  3. 3.

    The English-language texts were elicited in San Diego, California, under the supervision of Judy S. Reilly, and Hebrew data-collection was conducted in Israel by the author and her associates.

  4. 4.

    This trend is no doubt intensified by the particular sub-genre of narrative, a personal-experience account of a situation in which the narrators themselves were directly involved, but the subjective discourse stance provoked by this communicative context applies similarly in the written medium.

References

  • Allen, M., Kertoy, M., Sherblom, J., & Pettit, J. (1994). Children’s narrative productions: Comparing personal events and fictional stories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 149–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Ilan, L., & Berman, R. A. (2007). Developing register differentiation: The Latinate-Germanic divide in English. Linguistics, 45, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benson, M. S. (1993). 4- and 5-year olds’ narratives in pretend play and storytelling. First Language, 13, 203–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (1997). Narrative theory and narrative development: The Labovian impact. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 7, 235–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (2005). Introduction: Developing discourse stance in different text types and languages. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(2), 15–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (2007). Developing language knowledge and language use across adolescence. In E. Hoff & M. Shatz (Eds.), Handbook of language development (pp. 346–367). London: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (2008). The psycholinguistics of developing text construction. Journal of Child Language, 35, 735–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (2009a). Beyond the sentence: Language development in narrative contexts. In E. Bavin (Ed.), Handbook of child language (pp. 354–375). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (2009b). Linguistic, cognitive, and pragmatic issues in developing narrative abilities. In J. Guo, E. Lieven, S. Ervin-Tripp, N. Budwig, S. Özçalişkan, & K. Nakamura (Eds.), Crosslinguistic approaches to the psychology of language: Research in the tradition of Dan I. Slobin (pp. 119–126). New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (2011a). Revisiting impersonal constructions in Hebrew: Corpus-based perspectives. In A. Malchov & A. Sierwieska (Eds.), The typology of impersonal constructions (pp. 323–355). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (2011b). Language and literacy in adolescence. In J. R. Levesque (Ed.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (pp. 1320–1332). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Katzenberger, I. (2004). Form and function in introducing narrative and expository texts: A developmental perspective. Discourse Processes, 38, 57–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Nir, B. (2010a). The language of expository texts: Developmental perspectives. In M. Nippold & C. Scott (Eds.), Expository discourse in children, adolescents, and adults: Development and disorders (pp. 101–123). New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Nir, B. (2010b). The lexicon in speech-writing differentiation: Developmental perspectives. Written Language & Literacy, 13, 185–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Nir-Sagiv, B. (2004). Linguistic indicators of inter-genre differentiation in later language development. Journal of Child Language, 31, 339–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Nir-Sagiv, B. (2007). Comparing narrative and expository text construction across adolescence: A developmental paradox. Discourse Processes, 43, 79–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Nir-Sagiv, B. (2009a). Cognitive and linguistic factors in evaluating expository text quality: Global versus local? In V. Evans & S. Pourcel (Eds.), New directions in cognitive linguistics (pp. 421–440). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Nir-Sagiv, B. (2009b). Clause-packaging in narratives: A crosslinguistic developmental study. In J. Guo, E. Lieven, S. Ervin-Tripp, N. Budwig, S. Özçalişkan, & K. Nakamura (Eds.), Crosslinguistic approaches to the psychology of language: Research in the tradition of Dan I. Slobin (pp. 149–182). New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Nir-Sagiv, B. (2011). Spoken and written language across the school years. In D. Aram & O. Korat (Eds.), Literacy and language: Interactions, bilingualism and difficulties; Festschrift in Honor of Iris Levin (Vol. I, pp. 211–229). Jerusalem: Magnes Press [in Hebrew].

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Ravid, D. (2009). Becoming a literate language user: Oral and written text construction across adolescence. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of literacy (pp. 92–111). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2002). Developing text production abilities in speech and writing: Aims and methodology. Written Languages and Literacy, 5, 1–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., Ragnarsdóttir, H., & Strömqvist, S. (2002). Discourse stance: Written and spoken language across development. Written Language & Literacy, 5(2), 255–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biber, D. (1988). Variation across spoken and written English. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Biber, D. (1995). Dimensions of register variation: A crosslinguistic comparison. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Blum-Kulka, S. (2004). The role of peer interaction in later pragmatic development: The case of speech representation. In R. A. Berman (Ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 191–210). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdin, B., & Fayol, M. (1994). Is written language production more difficult than oral language production? A working memory approach. International Journal of Psychology, 29, 591–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bushnell, P. P. (1930). An analytic contrast of oral with written language (Vol. 451, pp. 87–95). New York: Bureau of Publications: Teachers College Contributions to Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W. L. (1994). Discourse, consciousness, and time: The flow of language in speech and writing. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W. L., & Danielewicz, J. (1987). Properties of spoken and written language. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Comprehending oral and written language (pp. 83–112). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Wasow, T. (1998). Repeating words in spontaneous speech. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 201–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cleland, A. A., & Pickering, M. J. (2006). Do writing and speaking employ the same syntactic representations? Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 185–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, P. T. (1996). Introduction: The study of writing systems. In P. T. Daniels & W. Bright (Eds.), The world’s writing systems (pp. 1–18). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, P. T. (2002). The study of writing in the twentieth century: Semitic studies interacting with non-Semitic. In S. Izre’el (Ed.), Israel oriental studies 20: Semitic linguistics: The state of the art at the turn of the twenty-first century (pp. 85–111). Winona Lake: Eisenbrauns.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidi, O., & Berman, R. A. (2014). Writing abilities of pre-adolescents with and without language/learning impairment in restructuring an informative text. In B. Arfé, J. Dockrell, & V. Berninger (Eds.), Writing development in children with hearing loss, dyslexia, or oral language problems (pp. 143–157). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • DeVito, J. A. (1967). Levels of abstraction in spoken and written language. Journal of Communication, 17, 354–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drieman, G. H. (1962). Differences between written and spoken language. Acta Psychologica, 20, 36–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Du Bois, J. W. (1980). Beyond definiteness: The trace of identity in discourse. In W. Chafe (Ed.), The pear stories: Cognitive, cultural, and linguistic aspects of narrative production (pp. 74–203). Norwood: Alex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Du Bois, J. W. (2007). The stance triangle. In R. Englebretson (Ed.), Stancetaking in discourse: Subjectivity, evaluation, interaction (pp. 39–182). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eggins, S., & Martin, J. R. (1997). Genres and registers of discourse. In T. A. van Dijk (Ed.), Discourse as structure and process (pp. 230–256). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gee, J. (2006). Oral discourse in a world of literacy. Research in the Teaching of English, 41, 153–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giora, R. (1990). On the so-called evaluative material in informative text. Text, 4, 299–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1992). Rethinking context: An introduction. In C. Goodwin & A. Duranti (Eds.), Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon (pp. 1–42). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimshaw, A. D. (2003). Genres, registers, and contexts of discourse. In A. C. Graesser, M. A. Gernsbacher, & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), Handbook of discourse processes (pp. 25–82). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K. (1989). Spoken and written language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hicks, D. (1991). Kinds of narrative: Genre skills among first graders from two communities. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 55–88). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, J. A., & Shapiro, L. R. (1991). From knowing to telling: The development of children’s scripts, stories, and personal narratives. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 55–88). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jisa, H. (2004). Growing into academic French. In R. A. Berman (Ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 135–162). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jisa, H. (2005). Developing alternatives for indicating discourse stance. In D. Ravid & H. Shyldkrot (Eds.), Perspectives on language and language development: Essays in honor of Ruth A. Berman (pp. 357–374). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jisa, H., & Tolchinsky, L. (2009). Developing a depersonalised discourse stance in typologically different languages: Written expository texts. Written Language and Literacy, 12, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jisa, H., & Viguié, A. (2005). A developmental perspective on the role of French ‘on’ in written and spoken expository texts. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 125–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jisa, H., Reilly, J., Verhoeven, L., Baruch, E., & Rosado, E. (2002). Passive voice constructions in written texts. Written Language & Literacy, 5, 163–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, V. (2009). Developmental aspects of text production in writing and speech. Lund: Travaux de l’Institut de Linguistique de Lund.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, K. (1992). Beyond modularity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kress, G. (2002). Literacy in the new media age. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kress, G. (2010). Multimodality: A social semiotic approach to contemporary communication. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, E., Torrance, N. G., & Olson, D. (2001). Young children and the say/mean distinction: Verbatim and paraphrase recognition in narrative and nursery rhyme contexts. Journal of Child Language, 28, 531–543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lull, H. C. (1929). The speaking and writing abilities of intermediate grade pupils. Journal of Educational Resources, 20, 73–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maggio, S., Lété, B., Chenu, F., Jisa, H., & Fayol, M. (2012). Tracking the mind during writing: Immediacy, delayed, and anticipatory effects on pause and writing rate. Reading and Writing, 25, 2131–2151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malvern, D. M., Chipere, N., Richards, B. J., & Durán, P. (2004). Lexical diversity and language development. London: Palgrave Macmillan Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. R. (1983). The development of register. In J. Fine & R. O. Freedle (Eds.), Developmental issues in discourse (pp. 1–40). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazur-Palandre, A. (2009). Le flux de l’information, aspects syntaxiques et discursifs. Une étude fonctionnaliste et développementale. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Université Lyon 2, Lyon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazur-Palandre, A., & Jisa, H. (2012). La complexité lexicale des syntagmes nominaux: Une étude développementale. Enfance, 4, 359–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ninio, A., & Snow, C. (1996). Pragmatic development. Boulder: Westview.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nir, B., Bar-Ilan, L., & Berman, R. A. (2008). Vocabulary development across adolescence: Text-based analyses. In A. Stavans & I. Kupferberg (Eds.), Studies in language and language education: Essays in honor of Elite Olshtain (pp. 47–74). Jerusalem: Magnes Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. R. (1994). The world on paper: The conceptual and cognitive implications of writing and reading. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. R. (2006). Continuing the discourse on literacy. Research in Teaching of English, 41, 175–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ong, W. (1982). Orality and literacy: The technologizing of the word. London/New York: Methuen.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Paltridge, B. (2002). Genre, text type, and the English for Academic Purposes (EAP) classroom. In A. M. Johns (Ed.), Genre in the classroom: Multiple perspectives (pp. 73–90). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pappas, C. C., & Pettigrew, B. S. (1998). The role of genre in the psycholinguistic guessing game of reading. Language Arts, 75, 36–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D., & Galda, L. (1998). The development of school-based literacy. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ragnarsdóttir, H., & Strömqvist, S. (2005). The development of generic maður/man for the construction of discourse stance in Icelandic and Swedish. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 143–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ragnarsdóttir, H., Aparici, M., Cahana-Amitay, D., van Hell, J., & Viguié, A. (2002). Verbal structure and content in written discourse: Narrative and expository texts. Written Language and Literacy, 5, 95–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Berman, R. A. (2006). Information density in the development of spoken and written narratives in English and Hebrew. Discourse Processes, 41, 117–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Berman, R. A. (2009). Developing linguistic register across text types: The case of Modern Hebrew. Pragmatics and Cognition, 17, 108–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Berman, R. A. (2010). Developing noun phrase complexity across adolescence: A text-embedded analysis. First Language, 30, 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Tolchinsky, L. (2002). Developing linguistic literacy: A comprehensive model. Journal of Child Language, 29, 417–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., van Hell, J., Rosado, E., & Zamora, A. (2002). Subject NP patterning in the development of written and spoken text production. Written Language and Literacy, 5, 69–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, J. S., Baruch, E., Jisa, H., & Berman, R. A. (2002). Propositional attitudes: Development of modal expression. Written Language and Literacy, 5, 182–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, J. S., Zamora, A., & McGivern, R. F. (2005). Acquiring perspective in English: The development of stance. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 185–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosado, E., Salas, N., Aparici, M., & Tolchinsky, L. (2014). Production and judgment of linguistic devices for attaining a detached stance in Spanish and Catalan. Journal of Pragmatics, 60, 36–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salas, N. (2010). Developmental and discursive underpinnings in the expression of Spanish NPs. Diploma de Estudios Avanzados. Barcelona: University of Barcelona.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandbank, A. (2002). The interplay between the written language and writing conventions in writing texts. In L. Tolchinsky (Ed.), Developmental aspects of learning to write (pp. 55–76). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, C. M. (2012). Learning to write. In A. Kamhi & H. Catts (Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (3rd ed., pp. 244–268). Boston: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1996). From “thought and language” to “thinking to speaking”. In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 70–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (2003). Language and thought online: Cognitive consequences of linguistic relativity. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the investigation of language and thought (pp. 157–191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (2005). Relating events in translation. In D. Ravid & H. B. Shyldkrot (Eds.), Perspectives on language and language development: Essays in honor of Ruth A. Berman (pp. 115–129). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. S. (2003). Modes of discourse: The local structure of texts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Steen, J. G. (1999). Genres of discourse and the definition of literature. Discourse Processes, 28, 109–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strömqvist, S. (2006). Learning to write: A window on language, communication, and cognition. In J. Bernicot (Ed.), Pragmatique Développementale: Perspectives Européennes, special edition of Le langage et l’Homme (Vol. 41).

    Google Scholar 

  • Strömqvist, S., Johansson, V., Kriz, S., Ragnarsdóttir, H., Aisenman, R., & Ravid, D. (2002). Toward a cross-linguistic comparison of lexical quanta in speech and writing. Written Language & Literacy, 5, 45–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strömqvist, S., Nordqvist, Å., & Wengelin, Å. (2004). Writing the frog story: Developmental and cross-modal perspectives. In S. Strömqvist & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives (pp. 359–394). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swales, J. M. (1990). Genre analysis: English in academic and research settings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannen, D. (Ed.). (1982). Spoken and written language: Exploring orality and literacy. Advances in discourse processes (Vol. IX). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L. (2003). The cradle of culture: What children know about writing and numbers before being taught. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L. (2004). The nature and scope of later language development. In R. A. Berman (Ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 233–247). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L., & Rosado, E. (2005). The effect of literacy, text type, and modality on the use of grammatical means for agency alternation in Spanish. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(2), 209–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L., & Sandbank, A. (1994). Text production and text differentiation: Developmental changes and educational influences. In S. Strauss (Ed.), Learning environments and psychological development. Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L., & Teberosky, A. (1997). Explicit word segmentation and writing in Hebrew and Spanish. In C. Pontecorvo (Ed.), Writing development: An interdisciplinary view (pp. 77–98). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L., & Teberosky, A. (1998). The development of word segmentation and writing in two scripts. Cognitive Development, 13, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L., Johansson, V., & Zamora, A. (2002). Text openings and closings: Textual autonomy and differentiation. Written Language and Literacy, 5, 219–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ruth A. Berman .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Berman, R.A. (2016). Linguistic Literacy and Later Language Development. In: Perera, J., Aparici, M., Rosado, E., Salas, N. (eds) Written and Spoken Language Development across the Lifespan. Literacy Studies, vol 11. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-21136-7_12

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-21136-7_12

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-21135-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-21136-7

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics