Skip to main content

Cognitive Development and the Life Course: Growth, Stability and Decline

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research ((HSSR))

Abstract

This chapter explores the integration of life course and developmental approaches to the understanding of cognitive growth, maintenance, and decline over the life span. We employ the idea of event-centered growth models as an essential contribution that at once brings more contextual factors into developmental models of cognitive function (CF) and shows how the life course approach can contribute to an understanding of a phenomenon that is otherwise viewed as a developmental (or aging) process. After introducing the concepts, theories, and methods necessary for understanding the main focus of our attention—namely within-person change in CF—we apply this framework to four major areas where our approach can potentially lead to new insights and understandings: (1) early child cognitive development and the transition to school; (2) the transition to adulthood and midlife, in which CF in adolescence leads to major influences on educational achievement, occupational success and CF in adulthood; (3) the potential for changes in CF during midlife; and (4) transitions in later life (e.g. health events and labor force transitions),and how theoretically CF has a role to play as both consequence and cause of these transitions. The approach we propose can be applied to the study of a wide array of developmental phenomena where life course events and transitions play a role.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   99.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    One exception is Newcomb’s Bennington studies (see Alwin et al. 1991), which studied political attitudes over nearly 50 years in the lives of their respondents. Another exception is the Terman study of gifted children (Friedman and Kern 2014; Kern and Friedman 2008).

  2. 2.

    The molar stability coefficient is an estimate of the persistence of behavior or behavioral orientations as expressed in age-homogenous rates of change over specified periods of time. The concept was introduced as a means of organizing empirical information on human constancy and change, and of comparing raw stability estimates across studies having different re-measurement intervals and across different concepts (see Alwin 1994, pp. 155–158; Alwin 1995, pp. 233–238). As distinct from other concepts of stability (see Alwin 1994), molar stability is defined as βj/k where β is the cohort-specific or age-homogenous stability estimate observed empirically, k is the number of years over which raw stability is assessed, and j is the number of years selected to express molar stability. In the examples used in this chapter, j = 8.

  3. 3.

    In this discussion normative stability refers to the preservation of individual differences in a quality within a constant population over a specified amount of time, whereas molar stability refers to the persistence of a quality as expressed in the rate of change for an age-homogenous cohort over a specified period of time. As used here, the term normative stability simply refers to molar stability in the entire population, not broken down by cohort (see Alwin 1994, p. 139).

  4. 4.

    Because of the limitations on space, we do not discuss these models in detail here and introduce them primarily to provide a conceptual orientation to the study of within-person change, the central concept used throughout this chapter. Further discussion of these models for those uninitiated into the study of within-person change and the SEM approach may wish to consult introductory material on change models (see, e.g. Alwin n.d.).

  5. 5.

    Todd and Wolpin (2003) identify a useful example using a model in which child achievement is regressed on family income, the number of books in the home, and additional covariates. The issue arises with the interpretation of the estimated effects—increasing the number of books, while holding family income constant, implies a reduction in some other area of family consumption which may include child investment (e.g., fewer educational toys; assuming these inputs are purchased). Thus, the estimated effect is potentially confounded with a change in the level of an additional (unobserved) input.

  6. 6.

    We employ structural equation methods in this analysis, and focus on the total rather than direct effects of variables in a causal model (see Alwin 1988). The details of our treatment of the WLS data, including the measures and modeling strategies used, are given in Alwin et al. (2008a).

References

  • Aikens, N. L., & Barbarin, O. (2008). Socioeconomic differences in reading trajectories: The contribution of family, neighborhood, and school contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 235–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F. (1988). Structural equation models in research on human development and aging. In K. W. Schaie, R. T. Campbell, W. Meredith, & S. C. Rawlings (Eds.), Methodological issues in aging research (pp. 71–170). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F. (1994). Aging, personality and social change: The stability of individual differences over the adult life-span. In D. L. Featherman, R. M. Lerner, & M. Perlmuter (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 135–185). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F. (2008). Social structure and cognitive change. In S. M. Hofer & D. F. Alwin (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 418–444). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F. (2009). History, Cohorts, and Cognitive Aging. In H. B. Bosworth & C. Hertzog (Eds.), Aging and cognition: Research methodologies and empirical advances [Lead chapter in a Festschrift for K. Warner Schaie] (pp. 9–28). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F. (2010). Social structure and cognitive ageing. In D. Dannefer & C. Phillipson (Eds.), International handbook of social gerontology (pp. 265–279). London: Sage Publications International.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F. (2012). Integrating varieties of life course concepts. “Symposium on the Life Course”. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67B, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F. (n.d). A primer in the use of growth models to study within-person change. Unpublished manuscript. Center for Life Course and Longitudinal Studies, Pennsylvania State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F., & Hofer, S. M. (2011). Health and cognition in aging research. In D. F., Alwin, & S. M., Hofer (Eds.), Cognition, health and aging: Integrating perspectives across disciplines. Supplement to The Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66, 9–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F., & McCammon, R. J. (1999). Aging vs. cohort interpretations of intercohort differences in GSS verbal scores. American Sociological Review, 64, 272–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F., & McCammon, R. J. (2001). Aging, cohorts, and verbal ability. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 56B, S1–S11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F., & Thornton, A. (1984). Family origins and the schooling process: Early vs. late influence of parental characteristics. American Sociological Review, 49, 784–802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F., Cohen, R. L., & Newcomb, T. M. (1991). Political attitudes over the life span: The Bennington women after fifty years. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F., Hofer, S. M., & McCammon, R. J. (2006). Modeling the effects of time: Integrating demographic and developmental perspectives. In R. H. Binstock & L. K. George (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the social sciences (pp. 20–38). New York: Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F., Wray, L. A., & McCammon, R. J. (2008a). What determines individual differences in intellectual functioning in midlife. Center for Life Course and Longitudinal Studies, Penn State University. Unpublished paper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F., McCammon, R. J., Wray, L. A., & Rodgers, W. L. (2008b). Population processes and cognitive aging. In S. M. Hofer & D. F. Alwin (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 69–89). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Alwin, D. F., & others (n.d). The stability of individual differences in cognitive functioning over the life span. Unpublished manuscript. Center for Life Course and Longitudinal Studies, Pennsylvania State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alzheimer’s Disease International (ADI). (2014). World Alzheimer’s report 2014: Dementia and risk reduction: An analysis of protective and modifiable risk factors. London: ADI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anstey, K. J., Hofer, S. M., & Luszcz, M. A. (2003). A latent growth curve analysis of late-life sensory and cognitive function over 8 years: Evidence for specific and common factors underlying change. Psychology and Aging, 18, 714–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny: Selection, optimization and compensation as the foundation of developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52, 366–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. (1980). Plasticity and variability in psychological aging: Methodological and theoretical issues. In G. E. Gurski (Ed.), Determining the effects of aging on the central nervous system (pp. 41–66). Berlin: Schering.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1–34). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., & Mayer, K. U. (1999). The Berlin aging study: Aging from 70 to 100. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Lifespan psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning. In Annual review of psychology, 50 (pp. 471–507). Palo Alto: Annual Reviews.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, D. J. P. (1998). Mothers, babies, and health in later life. London: Churchill Livingstone.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnsley, R. H., & Thompson, A. H. (1988). Birthdate and success in minor hockey: The key to the NHL. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 20, 167–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnsley, R. H., Thompson, A. H., & Barnsley, P. E. (1985). Hockey success and birthdate: The relative age effect. Canadian Association for Health, Physical Education, and Recreation, 51, 23–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumert, J., Nagy, G., & Lehmann, R. (2012). Cumulative advantages and the emergence of social and ethnic inequality: Matthew effects in reading and mathematics development within elementary schools? Child Development, 83(4), 1347–1367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, L. M., Paxson, C., & Waldfogel, J. (2009). Income and child development. Child Youth Services Review, 31(9), 978–989.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blau, D., & Currie, J. (2006). Pre-school, day care, and after-school care: Who’s minding the kids? In E. A. Hanushek & F. Welch (Eds.), Handbook on the economics of education (Vol. 2). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blau, P. M., & Duncan, O. D. (1967). The American occupational structure. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, B. S. (1964). Stability and change in human characteristics. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodovski, K., & Farkas, G. (2008). ‘Concerted cultivation’ and unequal achievement in elementary school. Social Science Research, 37, 903–919.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bollen, K. A., & Curran, P. J. (2006). Latent curve models: A structural equation perspective. Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bound, J., Schoenbaum, M., Stinebrickner, T. R., & Waidmann, T. (1999). The dynamic effects of health on the labor force transitions of older workers. Labour Economics, 6, 179–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S., Gintis, H., & Groves, M. O. (2005). Unequal chances: Family background and economic success. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 371–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101, 568–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brookmeyer, R., & Gray, S. (2000). Methods for projecting the incidence and prevalence of chronic diseases in ageing populations: Application to Alzheimer’s disease. Statistics in Medicine, 19, 1481–1493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brookmeyer, R., Gray, S., & Kawas, C. (1998). Projections of Alzheimer’s disease in the United States and the public health impact of delaying disease onset. American Journal of Public Health, 88, 1337–1342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cahill, K. E., Giandrea, M. D., & Quinn, J. F. (2006). Retirement patterns from career employment. The Gerontologist, 46, 514–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, J. R. (2003). Longevity: The biology and demography of life span. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (1996). Mathematical abilities: Some results from factor analysis. In R. J. Sternberg & R. Ben-Zeev (Eds.), The nature of mathematical thinking (pp. 3–25). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (1998). Human cognitive abilities. In J. J. McArdle & R. W. Woodcock (Eds.), Human cognitive abilities in theory and practice (pp. 5–24). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. (1971a). The structure of intelligence in relation to the nature-nurture controversy. In R. Cancro (Ed.), Intelligence: Genetic and environmental influences (pp. 3–30). New York: Grune and Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. (1971b). Abilities: Their structure, growth and action. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheadle, J. E. (2008). Educational investment, family context, and children’s math and reading growth from kindergarten through the third grade. Sociology of Education, 81, 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Condron, D. J. (2009). Social class, school and non-school environments, and black/white inequalities in children’s learning. American Sociological Review, 74(5), 683–708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cosway, R., Strachan, M. W. J., Dougall, A., Frier, B. M., & Deary, I. J. (2001). Cognitive function and information processing in type 2 diabetes. Diabetic Medicine, 18, 803–810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crimmins, E. M. (2001). Mortality and health in human life spans. Experimental Gerontology, 36, 885–897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crimmins, E. M. (2005). Socioeconomic differentials in mortality and health at the older ages. Genus, 61, 163–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crimmins, E. M., Hayward, M. D., & Seeman, T. (2004). Race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and health. In N. Anderson, R. Bulatao, & B. Cohen (Eds.), Critical perspectives on race and ethnic differences in health in later life (pp. 310–352). Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How should we measure “change” – Or should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74, 8–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crooks, V. C., Buckwalter, J. G., & Petitti, D. B. (2003). Diabetes mellitus and cognitive performance in older women. Annals of Epidemiology, 13, 613–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunha, F., & Heckman, J. J. (2007). The technology of skill formation. American Economic Review, 97, 31–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunha, F., & Heckman, J. J. (2008). Formulating, identifying and estimating the technology of cognitive and noncognitive skill formation. Journal of Human Resources, 43, 738–782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curran, P. J., & Bollen, K. A. (2001). The best of both worlds: Combining autoregressive and latent curve models. In L. M. Collins & A. G. Sayer (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 105–135). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Currie, J., & Thomas, D. (1995). Does head start make a difference? American Economic Review, 85, 341–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dannefer, W. D. (2003). Cumulative advantage/disadvantage and the life course: Cross-fertilizing age and social science theory. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58, S327–S357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis-Kean, P. E. (2005). The influence of parent education and family income on child achievement: The indirect role of parental expectations and the home environment. Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 294–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Kieviet, J. F., Zoetebier, L., Van Elburg, R. M., Vermeulen, R. J., & Oosterlaan, J. (2012). Brain development of very preterm and very Low-birthweight children in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 54, 313–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denny, N. W. (1982). Aging and cognitive change. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychology: Research and theory (pp. 807–827). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318, 1387–1388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiPrete, T. A., & Eirich, G. M. (2006). Cumulative advantage as a mechanism for inequality: A review of theoretical and empirical developments. Annual Review of Sociology, 32, 271–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downey, D. B., von Hippel, P. T., & Broh, B. A. (2004). Are schools the great equalizer? Cognitive inequality during the summer months and the school year. American Sociological Review, 69(5), 613–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, O. D., Featherman, D. I., & Duncan, B. (1972). Socioeconomic background and achievement. New York: Seminar Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, G., Kalil, A., Mayer, S. E., Tepper, R., & Payne, M. R. (2005). The apple does not fall far from the tree. In S. Bowles, H. Gintis, & M. O. Groves (Eds.), Unequal chances: Family background and economic success (pp. 23–79). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., Pagani, L. S., Feinstein, L., Engel, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., Sexton, H., Duckwork, K., & Jape, C. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1428–1446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elder, G. H., Jr. (1975). Age differentiation and the life course. Annual Review of Sociology, 1, 165–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elder, G. H., Jr. (1985). Perspectives on the life course. In G. H. Elder Jr. (Ed.), Life course dynamics: Trajectories and transitions, 1968–1980 (pp. 23–49). Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder, G. H., Jr. (1997). The life course and human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (pp. 939–991). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder, G. H., Jr. (2000). The life course. In E. F. Borgatta & R. J. V. Montgomery (Eds.), Encyclopedia of sociology (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 1614–1622). New York: Macmillan Reference USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder, G. H., Jr. (1997a, April 5). The life course as developmental theory. Presidential address, Society for Research on Child Development, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder, G. H., Jr., & Shanahan, M. J. (2006). The life course and human development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Theoretical models of human development 6th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 665–715). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder, G. H., Jr., Johnson, M. K., & Crosnoe, R. (2003). The emergence and development of life course theory. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the life course (pp. 3–19). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Entwistle, D., Alexander, K. L., & Olson, L. S. (2003). The first grade transition in life course perspective. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the life course (pp. 229–250). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Farkas, G., & Beron, K. (2004). The detailed age trajectory of oral vocabulary knowledge: Differences by race and class. Social Science Research, 33, 464–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Featherman, D. L. (1983). Life-span perspectives in social science research. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 5, pp. 1–57). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. W. (1985). Ontogenesis and sociogenesis: Problematics for theory and research about development and socialization across the life span. American Sociological Review, 50, 659–676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferraro, K. F., & Shippee, T. P. (2009). Aging and cumulative inequality: How does inequality get under the skin? The Gerontologist, 49, 333–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finch, C. E., & Kirkwood, T. B. L. (2000). Chance, development and aging. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan, D. P., McGrew, K. S., & Ortiz, S. O. (2000). The Wechsler intelligence scales and Gf-Gc theory: A contemporary approach to interpretation. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, H. S., & Kern, M. L. (2014). Personality, well-being, and health. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 719–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijters, P., Johnston, D. W., Shah, M., & Shields, M. A. (2013). Intrahousehold resource allocation: Do parents reduce or reinforce child ability gaps? Demography, 50, 2187–2208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R. (1990). Structural constraints on cognitive development: Introduction to a special issue of cognitive science. Cognitive Science, 14, 3–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geroldi, C., Ferrucci, L., Bendinelli, S., Cavazzini, C., Zanetti, O., Guralnik, J. M., & Frisoni, G. B. (2003). Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 51, 1064–1071.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gershoff, E. T., Aber, J. L., Raver, C. C., & Lennon, M. C. (2007). Income is not enough: Incorporating material hardship into models of income associations with parenting and child development. Child Development, 78, 70–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giedd, J. N. (2008). The teen brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42, 335–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijenbos, A., Paus, T., Evans, A. C., & Rapoport, J. L. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 861–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gladwell, M. (2008). Outliers—the story of success. New York: Little Brown & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, R. L., & Landy, D. (2010). Domain-creating constraints. Cognitive Science, 34, 1357–1377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (2012). Scientific thinking in young children: Theoretical advances, empirical research, and policy implications. Science, 337, 1623–1627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1998). Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, MA: The MIT University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson, L. S., & Deary, I. J. (2004). Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guo, G., & Harris, K. M. (2000). The mechanisms mediating the effects of poverty on children’s intellectual development. Demography, 37(4), 431–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanushek, E. A., & Maritato, N. L. (Eds.). (1996). Assessing knowledge of retirement behavior. Panel on Retirement Income Modeling. Committee on Statistics. Commisson on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. National Research Council. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. W. (1963). Problems in measuring change. Madison: The University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child’s environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hassing, L. B., Johansson, B., Pedersen, N. L., Nilsson, S. E., Berg, S., & McClearn, G. (2003). Type 2 diabetes mellitus and cognitive performance in a population-based sample of the oldest old: Impact of comorbid dementia. Aging, Neuropsychology, & Cognition, 10, 99–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hassing, L. B., Grant, M. D., Hofer, S. M., Pedersen, N. L., Nilsson, S. E., Berg, S., McClearn, G. E., & Johansson, B. (2004a). Type 2 diabetes mellitus contributes to cognitive change in the oldest old: A longitudinal population-based study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 599–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hassing, L. B., Hofer, S. M., Nilsson, S. E., Berg, S., Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G., & Johansson, B. (2004b). Comorbid type 2 diabetes mellitus and hypertension exacerbates cognitive decline: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Age and Aging, 33, 355–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, R. M., Tsai, S.-L., & Sewell, W. H. (1983). A model of stratification with response error in social and psychological variables. Sociology of Education, 56, 20–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, R. M., Sewell, W. H., Logan, J. A., Hauser, T. S., Ryff, C., Caspi, A., & MacDonald, M. (1992). The Wisconsin longitudinal study: Adults as parents and children at age 50. IASSIST Quarterly, 16, 23–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, R. M., Carr, D., Hauser, T. S., Krecker, M., Kuo, D., Presit, D., Shinberg, D., Sweeney, M., Thompson-Colon, T., & Uhrig, S. C. N. (1993). The class of 1957 after 35 years: Overview and preliminary findings (CDE Working Paper 93-17). Madison: University of Wisconsin-Madison.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayward, M. D., Crimmins, E. M., Miles, T., & Yang, Y. (2000). The significance of socio-economic status in explaining the race gap in chronic health. American Sociological Review, 65, 910–930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckman, J. J. (2007). The economics, technology, and neuroscience of human capability formation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 13250–13255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heise, D. R. (1970). Causal inference from panel data. In Sociological methodology 1970 (pp. 3–27). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henretta, J. (2003). The life-course perspective on work and retirement. In R. A. Settersten Jr. (Ed.), Invitation to the life course: Toward new understandings of later life (pp. 85–105). Amityville: Baywood Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., & Schaie, K. W. (1986). Stability and change in adult intelligence. 1: Analysis of longitudinal covariance structures. Psychology and Aging, 1, 159–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., & Schaie, K. W. (1988). Stability and change in adult intelligence: 2. Simultaneous analysis of longitudinal means and covariance structures. Psychology and Aging, 3, 122–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, S. M., & Alwin, D. F. (2008). Handbook on cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, D. P. (1981). Transitions and social change: The early lives of American men. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, D. P. (2000). Life cycle. In E. F. Borgatta & R. J. V. Montgomery (Eds.), Encyclopedia of sociology (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 1623–1627). New York: Macmillan Reference USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, D. P., & Astone, N. M. (1986). The transition to adulthood. Annual Review of Sociology, 12, 109–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J. L. (1968). Organization of abilities and the development of intelligence. Psychological Review, 75, 242–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J. L. (1976). Human abilities: A review of research and theory in the early 1970s. Annual Review of Psychology, 27, 437–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J. L. (1994). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human intelligence (pp. 443–451). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1967). Age differences in fluid and crystallized intelligence. Acta Psychologica, 26, 107–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J. L., & Donaldson, G. (1980). Cognitive development in adulthood. In O. G. Brim Jr. & J. Kagan (Eds.), Constancy and change in human development (pp. 445–529). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

  • House, J. S., Kessler, R. C., Herzog, A. R., Mero, R. P., Kinney, A. M., & Breslow, M. J. (1992). Social stratification, age, and health. In K. W. Schaie, D. Blazer, & J. S. House (Eds.), Aging, health behaviors and health outcomes (pp. 1–32). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • House, J. S., Lepkowski, J. M., Kinney, A. M., Mero, R. P., Kessler, R. C., & Herzog, A. R. (1994). The social stratification of aging and health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 35, 213–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • House, J. S., Lantz, P. M., & Herd, P. (2005). Continuity and change in the social stratification of aging and health over the life course: Evidence from a nationally representative longitudinal study from 1986 to 2001/2002 (Americans’ Changing Lives Study). Journal of Gerontology: Social Science, 60B(Special Issue II), 15–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hox, J. J. (2002). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jencks, C. (1979). Who gets ahead? The determinants of economic success in America. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. B., Blum, R. W., & Giedd, J. N. (2009). Adolescent maturity and the brain: The promise and pitfalls of neuroscience research in adolescent health policy. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45, 216–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jöreskog, K. G. (1974). Analyzing psychological data by structural analysis of covariance matrices. In D. H. Kranz, R. C. Atkinson, R. D. Luce, & P. Suppes (Eds.), Measurement, psychophysics and neural information processing. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Magdalena Hurtado, A. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, G. A., Turrell, G., Lynch, J. W., Everson, S. A., Helkala, E. L., & Salonen, J. T. (2001). Childhood socioeconomic position and cognitive function in adulthood. International Journal of Epidemiology, 30, 256–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawas, C. H., & Brookmeyer, R. (2001). Editorial—aging and the public health: Effects of dementia. The New England Journal of Medicine, 344, 1160–1161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kern, M. L., & Friedman, H. S. (2008). Early educational milestones as predictors of lifelong academic achievement, midlife adjustment, and longevity. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 419–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knudsen, E. I., Heckman, J. J., Cameron, J. L., & Shonkoff, J. P. (2006). Economic, neurobiological, and behavioral perspectives on building America’s future workforce. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 10155–10162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohli, M. (2007). The institutionalization of the life course: Looking back to look ahead. Research in Human Development, 4, 253–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1978). The reciprocal effects of the substantive complexity of work and intellectual flexibility: A longitudinal assessment. American Journal of Sociology, 84, 24–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1983). Work and personality: An inquiry into the impact of social stratification. Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, M. L., & Slomczynski, K. M. (1990). Social structure and self-direction: A comparative analysis of the United States and Poland. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuh, D., & New Dynamics of Ageing (NDA) Preparatory Network. (2007). A life course approach to healthy aging, frailty, and capability. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 62A, 717–721.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunst, A. E., & Mackenbach, J. P. (1994). The size of mortality differences associated with education level in nine industrialized countries. American Journal of Public Health, 84, 932–937.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lareau, A., & Conley, D. (Eds.). (2008). Social class: How does it work? New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lebel, C., & Beaulieu, C. (2011). Longitudinal development of human brain wiring continues from childhood into adulthood. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(30), 10937–10947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, E. F. (1953). Design and analysis of experiments in psychology and education. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J. C. (1997). A test of J.R. Harris’s theory of peer influences on personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1197–1201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, F. M. (1963). Elementary models for measuring change. In C. W. Harris (Ed.), Problems in measuring change (pp. 21–38). Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, S. M. (2003). Cohort and life-course patterns in the relationship between education and health: A hierarchical approach. Demography, 40, 309–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical overview. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1006–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization and the context of the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Socialization, personality, and social development. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magnuson, K. A., Meyers, M. K., Ruhm, C. J., & Waldfogel, J. (2004). Inequality in preschool education and school readiness. American Educational Research Journal, 41(1), 115–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magnuson, K. A., Ruhm, C. J., & Waldfogel, J. (2007). The persistence of preschool effects: Do subsequent classroom experiences matter? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22, 18–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M. (2004). The foundations of mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marmot, M. G., & Shipley, M. J. (1996). Do socioeconomic differences in mortality persist after retirement? 25 year follow up of civil servants from the first Whitehall Study. British Medical Journal, 313, 1177–1180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, G. M., Austad, S. N., & Johnson, T. E. (1996). Genetic analysis of ageing: Role of oxidative damage and environmental stresses. Nature Genetics, 13, 25–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McArdle, J. J. (2001). A latent difference score approach to longitudinal dynamic structural analyses. In R. Cudeck, S. du Toit, & D. Sörbom (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: Present and future (pp. 342–380). Lincolnwood: Scientific Software International.

    Google Scholar 

  • McArdle, J. J., & Bell, R. Q. (2000). An introduction to latent growth models for developmental data analysis. In T. D. Little, K. U. Schnabel, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Modeling longitudinal and multilevel data: Practical issues, applied approaches and specific examples (pp. 69–107). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McArdle, J. J., & Hamagami, F. (2001). Latent difference score structural models for linear dynamic analyses with incomplete longitudinal data. In L. M. Collins & A. G. Sayer (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 137–175). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., & Seigler, R. S. (Eds.). (2001). Mechanisms of cognitive development: Behavioral and neural perspectives. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrew, K. S. (1997). Analysis of the major intelligence batteries according to a proposed comprehensive Gf-Gc framework. In D. P. Flanagan, J. L. Genshaft, & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (pp. 151–180). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrew, K. S., & Flanagan, D. P. (1998). The intelligence test desk reference (ITDR): Gf-Gc cross-battery assessment. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meredith, W., & Tisak, J. (1990). Latent curve analysis. Psychometrika, 55, 107–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merton, R. K. (1968). The Matthew effect in science. Science, 159, 56–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molla, M. T., Madans, J. H., & Wagener, D. K. (2004). Differentials in adult mortality and activity limitation by years of education in the United States at the end of the 1990s. Population and Development Review, 30, 625–646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, S. L., Grusky, D. B., & Fields, G. S. (2006). Mobility and inequality: Frontiers of research in sociology and economics. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muller, M., Sigurdsson, S., Kjartansson, O., et al. (2014). Birth size and brain function 75 years later. Pediatrics, 134, 761–770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Musch, J., & Grondin, S. (2001). Unequal competition as an impediment to personal development: A review of the relative age effect in sport. Developmental Review, 21, 147–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Musgrove, F. (1977). Margins of the mind. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2000). The relation of child care to cognitive and language development. Child Development, 71, 960–980.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2002a). Child-care structure -> process -> outcome: Direct and indirect effects of child-care quality on young children’s development. Psychological Science, 13(3), 199–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2002b). Early child care and children’s development prior to school entry: Results from the NICHD study of early child care. American Educational Research Journal, 39, 133–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2003). Does amount of time spent in child care predict socioemotional adjustment during the transition to kindergarten. Child Development, 74, 976–1005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Child care and child development: Results from the NICHD study of early child care and youth development. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Rand, A. M. (1996). The precious and the precocious: Understanding cumulative disadvantage and cumulative advantage over the life course. The Gerontologist, 36, 230–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostir, G. V., Maji, M. A., Ottenbacher, K. J., Markides, K. S., & Goodwin, J. S. (2003). Cognitive function and incidence of stroke in older Mexican Americans. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 58(6), 531–535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, D.C. (1994). Aging, cognition, and work. Human Performance, 7, 181–205

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, D. C. (1999). Cognitive aging, processing resources, and self-report. In D. C. Park & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognition, aging, and self-reports (pp. 45–69). Philadelphia: Taylor and Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, D. C., Smith, A. D., Lautenschlager, G., Earles, J., Frieske, D., Zwahr, M., & Gaines, C. (1996). Mediators of long-term memory performance across the life-span. Psychology and Aging, 11, 621–637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paus, T., Zijdenbos, A., Worsley, K., Collins, D. L., Blumenthal, J., Giedd, J. N., Rapoport, J. L., & Evans, A. C. (1999). Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: In vivo study. Science, 283, 1908–1911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlik, V. N., Alves de Moraes, S., Szklo, M., Knlpman, D. S., Mosely, T. H., Jr., & Hyman, D. J. (2003). Relation between cognitive function and mortality in middle-aged adults in the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 157, 327–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pedersen, N. L., & Lichtenstein, P. (1997). Biometric analyses of human abilities. In C. Cooper & V. Varma (Eds.), Processes in individual differences (pp. 126–148). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1965). Cognitive development in children: Piaget. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 2, 176–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (pp. 703–732). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget. (1985). The equilibration of cognitive structures: The central problem of intellectual development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R. (1994). Genetics and experience: The interplay between nature and nurture. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R. (1999). Behavioral genetics. In M. Bennett (Ed.), Developmental psychology: Achievements and prospects (pp. 231–252). Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potter, D., & Roksa, J. (2013). Accumulating advantage over time: Family experiences and social class inequality in academic achievement. Social Science Research, 42, 1018–1032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potter, D., Mashburn, A., & Grissmer, D. (2013). The family, neuroscience, and academic skills: An interdisciplinary account of social class gaps in children’s test scores. Social Science Research, 42, 446–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, M., & Deary, I. J. (2005). A life course approach to cognitive reserve: A model for cognitive aging and development? Annals of Neurology, 58, 617–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riley, M. W. (1987). On the significance of age in sociology. American Sociological Review, 52, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rindfuss, R. (1991). The young adult years: Diversity, structural change, and fertility. Demography, 28, 493–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robert, S., & House, J. S. (1996). SES differentials in health by age and alternative indicators of SES. Journal of Aging and Health, 8, 359–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogosa, D. R., Brandt, D., & Zimowski, M. (1982). A growth curve approach to the measurement of change. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 726–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, D. C. (1994). The limits of family influence: Genes, experience, and behavior. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1998). Successful aging. New York: Pantheon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, D. C., & Teachman, J. (2001). Behavioral genetic research designs and social policy studies. In A. Thornton (Ed.), The well-being of children and families: Research and data needs (pp. 157–187). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryder, N. B. (1965). The cohort as a concept in the study of social change. American Sociological Review, 30, 843–861.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T. A. (1999). Pressing issues in cognitive aging. In N. Schwarz, D. Park, B. Knäuper, & S. Sudman (Eds.), Cognition, aging, and self-reports (pp. 185–198). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development of individual differences. Child Development, 63, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarr, S. (1995). How do families affect intelligence? Social environmental and behavior genetic predictions. In J. J. Hox, B. F. van der Meulen, J. M. A. M. Janssens, J. J. F. ter Laak, & L. W. C. Tavecchio (Eds.), Advances in family research. Amsterdam: Thesis Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K. W. (2005). Developmental influences on adult intelligence: The Seattle longitudinal study. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Scherr, P. A., Albert, M. S., Funkenstein, H. H., Cook, N. R., Hennekens, C. H., Branch, L. G., White, L. R., Taylor, J. O., & Evans, D. A. (1988). Correlates of cognitive function in an elderly community population. American Journal of Epidemiology, 128, 1084–1101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, C. (1984). Psychological effects of complex environments during the life span: A review and theory. Intelligence, 8, 259–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, C. (1987). Psychological effects of complex environments during the life span: A review and theory. In C. Schooler & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Cognitive functioning and social structure over the lifecourse (pp. 24–49). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, C. (1998). Environmental complexity and the Flynn effect. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (pp. 67–79). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sewell, W. H., Sr., & Hauser, R. M. (1975). Education, occupation and earnings achievement in the early career. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sewell, W. H., Hauser, R. M., Springer, K. W., & Hauser, T. S. (2003). As we age: The Wisconsin longitudinal study, 1957–2001. In K. Leicht (Ed.), Social stratification and mobility (Vol. 20, pp. 3–111). London: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, M. J. (2000). Pathways to adulthood in changing societies: Variability and mechanisms in life course perspective. Annual Review of Sociology, 26, 667–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, M. J., Hofer, S. M., & Shanahan, L. (2003). Biological models of behavior and the life course. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the life course. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (Eds.). (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early child development. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. (1989). Mechanisms of cognitive development. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 353–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. London: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. R., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. K. (1997). Consequences of growing up poor, chap. Consequences of living in poverty for young children’s cognitive and verbal ability and early school achievement. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon, D. A., Kemper, S. J., Mortimer, J. A., Greiner, L. H., Wekstein, D. R., & Markesbery, W. R. (1996). Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer’s disease in late life: Findings from the Nun Study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 275, 528–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Resilience and reserve capacity in later adulthood: Potentials and limits of development across the life span. In D. Cicchetti & C. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (Risk, Disorder, and Adaptation, Vol. 2, pp. 801–847). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, Y. (Ed.). (2007). Cognitive reserve: Theory and applications. New York: Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, P. C., & Carstensen, L. L. (2000). The aging mind—Opportunities in cognitive research. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steyer, R., Eid, M., & Schwenkmezger, P. (1997). Methods of Psychological Research Online, 2(1), 21–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, P. E., & Wolpin, K. I. (2003). On the specification and estimation of the production function for cognitive achievement. Economic Journal, 113, F3–F33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Todd, P. E., & Wolpin, K. I. (2007). The production of cognitive achievement in children: Home, school, and racial test score gaps. Journal of Human Resources, 1, 91–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D’Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14(6), 623–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families. (2010). Head start impact study. Final report. Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Votruba-Drzal, E., Li-Grining, C. P., & Maldonado-Carreno, C. (2008). A developmental perspective on full- versus part-day kindergarten and children’s academic trajectories through fifth grade. Child Development, 79(4), 957–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1952). The range of human capacities. Baltimore: The Williams & Wilkins Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. Baltimore: The Williams & Wilkins Company.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Whalley, L. J., & Deary, I. J. (2001). Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and survival up to age 76. British Medical Journal, 322, 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willett, J. B., & Sayer, A. G. (1994). Using covariance structure analysis to detect correlates and predictors of individual change over time. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 363–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. S., de Leon Mendes, C. F., Bennett, C., Beenias, J. L., & Evans, D. A. (2004). Depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in a community population of older persons. Journal of Neurology and Neurosurgical Psychiatry, 75, 126–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1994). Measures of fluid and crystallized intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The encyclopedia of intelligence (pp. 452–456). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wray, L. A., Alwin, D. F., Zeiser, K. L., & Hofer, S. M. (2010). Does memory performance predict retirement in older workers. Unpublished Paper. Center for Life Course and Longitudinal Studies, Pennsylvania State University.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Duane F. Alwin .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Alwin, D.F., Thomas, J.R., Wray, L.A. (2016). Cognitive Development and the Life Course: Growth, Stability and Decline. In: Shanahan, M., Mortimer, J., Kirkpatrick Johnson, M. (eds) Handbook of the Life Course. Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-20880-0_21

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-20880-0_21

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-20879-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-20880-0

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics