Skip to main content

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Well-Being and Quality of Life Research ((BRIEFSWELLBEING))

  • 1177 Accesses

Abstract

In the recent decades, the most prominent theoretical framework to explain thinking concerns the dual process theories of cognition. These theories posit the existence of two separate cognitive systems, System 1 and System 2, that are in charge of autonomous and non-conscious cognition, and volitional and conscious cognition, respectively. The dual process theories form a strong basis to build a structural model of intuitive thought. Intuition is a form of cognition generated by ontogenetic System 1 processes, as differentiated from phylogenetic, or instinctive System 1 processes. Intuition is a form of skilled action, based on expertise. Intuition is a domain-specific capacity and thus highly context-sensitive, generated and activated by environmental and social cues. Thus the environment plays also a significant role in both the generation of intuitive cognitive processes as well as in cueing and priming the existing processes. I will argue, that in a structural model of intuitive thought the environment can be construed as a “System 3” that has direct cognitive bearing to the processes driven by Systems 1 and 2.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bargh, J. A. (2011). Unconscious thought theory and its discontents: A critique of the critiques. Social Cognition, 29(6), 629–647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (2012). Priming effects replicate just fine, thanks. Psychology Today.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54(7), 462–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 230–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Rediscovering the greatest human strength (Kindle ed.). New York: The Penguin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratlavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252–1265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L., & LePage, A. (1967). Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7(2), 202–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buschman, T. J., Siegel, M., Roy, J. E., & Miller, E. K. (2011). Neural substrates of cognitive capacity limitations. PNAS, 108(27).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, C. D., Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L., & Payne, B. K. (2012). Sequential priming measures of implicit social cognition: A meta-analysis of associations with behavior and explicit attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 61(4), 330–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., Ganellen, R. J., Froing, W. J., & Chambers, W. (1983). Modeling: An analysis in terms of category accessibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19(5), 403–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chabris, C., & Simons, D. (2010). The Invisible Gorilla: How Our Intuitions Deceive Us. New York: Crown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D. (2011). Foreword. In Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1999). An embodied cognitive science? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 345–351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2005). Intrinsic content, active memory and the extended mind. Analysis, 65, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2011). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 10–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: Harper Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dane, E., & Pratt, M. G. (2007). Exploring intuition and its role in managerial decision making. Academy of Management Review, 32(1), 33–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Neys, W., & Goel, V. (2011). Heuristics and biases in the brain: Dual neural pathways for decision making. In O. Vartanian & D. R. Mandel (Eds.), Neuroscience of decision making (pp. 125–141). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Decoster, J., & Claypool, H. M. (2004). A meta-analysis of priming effects on impression formation supporting a general model of informational biases. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(1), 2–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1997). Kinds of minds. Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich, A. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the experience of flow. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 746–761.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Djiksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: The merits of unconscious thought in preference development and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(5), 586–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Djiksterhuis, A., & Meurs, T. (2006). Where creativity resides: The generative power of unconscious thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 135–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Djiksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A theory of unconscious thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 95–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Draine, S. C., & Greenwald, A. G. (1998). Replicable unconscious semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(3), 286–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(19), 19–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (2002). Cognitive-experiential self-theory of personality. In Handbook of Psychology, Personality and Social Psychology (pp. 159–184).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Resch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100(3), 363–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Prietula, M. J., & Cokely, E. T. (2007). The Making of an Expert. Harvard Business Review, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(10), 454–459.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2009). How many dual-process theories do we need? One, two, or many? In J. S. B. T. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 33–54). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2010). Thinking twice. Chippenham/Eastbourne: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T., & Frankish, K. (2009). Preface. In J. S. B. T. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 5-7). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T., & Stanovich, K. (2013). Dual process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frankish, K., & Evans, J. S. B. T. (2009). The duality of mind: An historical perspective. In J. S. B. T. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 1–29). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books Inc, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (2007). Gut feelings: The intelligence of the unconscious (Kindle Edition). Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gladwell, M. (2005). Blink: The power of thinking without thinking. London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goel, V. (2007). Anatomy of deductie reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(10), 435–441.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hämäläinen, R. P., & Saarinen, E. (2008). Why systems intelligence? In R. P. Hämäläinen & E. Saarinen (Eds.), Systems intelligence: A new lens on human engagement and action (pp. vii–vix). Helsinki University of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heylighen, F., & Vidal, C. (2008). Getting things done: The science behind stress-free productivity. Long Range Planning, 41, 585–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huizenga, H. M., Wetzels, R., Ravenzwaaij, D. V., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2012). Four empirical tests of unconscious thought theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117, 332–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, J. G., Slone, L. B., Meteyer, K. B., & Matthews, A. R. (2002). The non-consciousness of self-consciousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 406–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., Lindsay, D. S., & Toth, J. P. (1992). Unconscious influences revealed: Attention, awareness and control. American Psychologist, 47(6), 808–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, R., & Hämäläinen, R. P. (2013). Esa saarinen and systems intelligence. In F. Martela, L. Järvilehto, P. Kenttä, & J. Korhonen (Eds.), Esa saarinen: Elämän filosofi (pp. 163–171). Aalto-yliopisto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Kindle Edition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2005). A model of heuristic judgment. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 267–293). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Klein, G. (2009). Conditions for intuitive expertise: A failure to disagree. American Psychologist, 64(6), 515–526.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, G. (1998). Sources of power: How people make decisions. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Intuition: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126(1), 109–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2009). What zombies can’t do: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to the irreducibility of reflective consciousness. In J. S. B. T. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 293–316). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Martela, M., & Saarinen, E. (2008). The nature of social systems in systems intelligence: Insights from intersubjetive systems theory. In R. P. Hämäläinen & E. Saarinen (Eds.), Systems intelligence: A new lens on human engagement and action (pp. 189–210). Helsinki University of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, H. N., & Mischel, W. (1983). The development of children’s knowledge of self-control strategies. Child Development, 54(3), 603–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rajahalme, J. (2008). David Bohm’s “thought as a system” and systems intelligence. In R. P. Hämäläinen & E. Saarinen (Eds.), Systems intelligence: A new lens on human engagement and action (pp. 29–38). Helsinki University of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, B. A., & Pickler, R. H. (2009). Mother-infant synchrony. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing, 38(4), 470–477.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saarinen, E., & Hämäläinen, R. P. (2004). Systems intelligence: Connecting engineering thinking with human sensitivity. In Systems intelligence—Discovering a hidden competence in human action and organizational life. Helsinki University of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saarinen, E., & Hämäläinen, R. P. (2010). The originality of systems intelligence. Essays on systems intelligence (pp. 9–26). Espoo: Aalto University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanks, D. R., Newell, B. R., Lee, E. H., Balakrishnan, D., Ekelund, L., Cenac, Z., et al. (2013). Priming intelligent behavior: An elusive phenomenon. PLoS ONE, 8(4), 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychological Review, 63(2), 129–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 1–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1992). What is an “explanation” of behavior? Psychological Science, 3(3), 150–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1996). The sciences of the artificial. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. (2004). Robot’s rebellion: Finding meaning in the age of Darwin. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. (2009). Distinguishing the reflective, algorithmic, and autonomous minds: Is it time for a tri-process theory? In J. S. B. T. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 55–88). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 646–726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C., & Evans, J. S. B. T. (1975). Dual processes in reasoning? Cognition, 3(2), 141–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (2004). Tractatus logico-philosophicus. Project Gutenberg.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lauri Järvilehto .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Järvilehto, L. (2015). The Nature of Intuitive Thought. In: The Nature and Function of Intuitive Thought and Decision Making. SpringerBriefs in Well-Being and Quality of Life Research. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-18176-9_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics