Skip to main content

Fear and Hope in Intractable Conflicts: The Automatic vs. Reflective Attributes of Collective Emotional Orientations

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Social Psychology of Intractable Conflicts

Part of the book series: Peace Psychology Book Series ((PPBS,volume 27))

Abstract

The main goal of this chapter is based on Bar-Tal’s concept of collective emotional orientation. The purpose is to point out the asymmetric nature of fear vs. hope in guiding people in context of intractable conflicts. Fear as a primary, holistic, and diffusive emotion of automatic origin can easily become a core affect of a collective emotional orientation. In contrast, hope, as an emotion of reflective origin can be shared only if a collective reaches a cognitive consensus about their aims and the types of activities that will lead to their realization. Such a consensus and the dominance of hope over fear in a situation of conflict can happen only if people are able to engage in reflective, evaluative reasoning. It depends on the development of the reflective–evaluative system, which can influence the innate, automatic ones. The duality of the evaluative systems is a necessary condition of the ability to perceive intractable conflicts from different perspectives.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 99.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Tal, D. (1989). Delegitimization: The extreme case of stereotyping and prejudice. In D. Bar-Tal, C. F. Graumann, A. W. Kruglanski, W. Stroebe (Eds.), Stereotyping and prejudices: Changing conceptions (pp. 169–182). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Tal, D. (2004). Nature, rational and effectiveness of education for co-existence. Journal of Social Issues, 60, 253–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Tal, D. (2013). Intractable conflicts. Socio-psychological foundations and dynamics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Tal, D. & Schnell, I. (Eds.) (2013). The impact of lasting occupation. Lessons from Israeli society. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., DeWall, C. N., & Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 167–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BÅ‚aszczak, W., Imbir, K. (2012). Use of affective priming to measure the implicit self-reference effect. Psychological Reports: Mental & Physical Health, 111, 107–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourgeois, P., Hess, U. (2008). The impact of social context on mimicry. Biological Psychology, 77, 343–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 191–214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, R., & Strack, F. (2006). Duality models in social psychology: From dual processes to interacting systems. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 166–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive-experiential self-theory. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 165–192). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halperin, E. (2014). Emotion, emotion regulation, and conflict resolution. Emotion Review, 6, 68–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imbir, K., & Jarymowicz, M. (2013).The effect of automatic vs. reflective emotions on cognitive control in the Antisaccade Tasks and the Emotional Stroop Test. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 44, 137–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. (1999). Positive affect and creativity. In S. Russ (Ed.), Affect, creative experience and psychological adjustment (pp. 3–17). Philadelphia: Brunner/Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K. & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 887–900.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jarymowicz, M. (2008). Psychologiczne podstawy podmiotowoÅ›ci [Psychological foundations of personal agency]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarymowicz, M. (2009). Emotions as evaluative processes: from primary affects to appraisal based on deliberative thinking. In A. BÅ‚achnio & A. Przepiórka (Eds.), Closer to Emotions (pp. 55–72). Lublin: Wydawnictwo KUL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarymowicz, M., & Bar-Tal, D. (2006). Dominance of fear over hope in the life of individuals and collectives. European Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 367–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarymowicz, M. & Imbir, K. (2015). Towards a human emotions taxonomy (based on their automatic vs. reflective origin). Emotion Review, 7, 183–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, M., Eyal, T., Meiran, N., Kessler, Y. (2010). Imagined positive emotions and inhibitory control: the differentiated effect of pride versus happiness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 36, 1314–1320.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1998). The emotional brain. New York, Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (2012). Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron, 73, 653–676.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, M. D. (2003). Reflexive and reflective judgment processes: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. In J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), Social judgments: Implicit and explicit processes (pp. 44–67). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Moya, P. M. L. (Eds.) (2010). Doing race. 21 essays for the 21st century. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 64, 723–739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding emotions. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1998). Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli: Expectances for aversive outcomes following nonrecognized fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 69–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohme, R. (2003). Podprogowe informacje mimiczne [The implicit facial signals]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo IP PAN i SWPS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1935). A brief outline of the higher nervous activity. In W. C. Murhison (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology: Psychologies in 1930. Worcester: Clark University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peeters, G., & CzapiÅ„ski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. In W. Stroebe, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology, v. 1 (pp. 33–60). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature. Why violence has declined. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: A psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reykowski, J. (1989). Dimensions of development of moral values. In N. Eisenberg, J. Reykowski, & E. Staub (Eds.), Social and moral values (pp. 23–44). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, E. T. (2000). Précis of the brain and emotion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 177–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sander, D., & Scherer, K. R. (Eds.) (2009). The Oxford companion to emotion and the affective sciences. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, C. R. (Ed.) (2000). Handbook of hope: Theory, measures, applications. San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (1989). Unintended thought. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across languages and cultures. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39, 117–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maria Jarymowicz .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Jarymowicz, M. (2015). Fear and Hope in Intractable Conflicts: The Automatic vs. Reflective Attributes of Collective Emotional Orientations. In: Halperin, E., Sharvit, K. (eds) The Social Psychology of Intractable Conflicts. Peace Psychology Book Series, vol 27. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-17861-5_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics