Advertisement

Input Versus Output in the Acquisition of Negative Polarity: The Curious Case of Any

  • Lyn TieuEmail author
Chapter
  • 931 Downloads
Part of the Language, Cognition, and Mind book series (LCAM, volume 1)

Abstract

This paper draws on the simple observation that young children acquire the constraints on negative polarity items (NPIs) with relative ease and speed, and, against the backdrop of existing theoretical proposals about licensing, identifies a fundamental learning puzzle. We will begin with an overview of the methods that have been used to tap into normally-developing English-speaking children’s knowledge of NPI any; such methods have revealed evidence of adult-like knowledge of the licensing condition on any in children as young as 2–3 years of age. To address the question of how children get to this stage, I examine samples of caregiver input, and discuss how they reveal different kinds of evidence for any’s restricted distribution and its licensers. Importantly however, I argue that the caregiver input does not provide direct evidence of the underlying semantics of any. If only a subset of what must be acquired is present in the input, we are left with a puzzling learning problem about how children arrive at the target representation of NPIs such as any.

Keywords

Negative polarity Acquisition Child language Learnability Child-directed speech NPI licensing 

Notes

Acknowledgments

For invaluable discussion, I am grateful to Emmanuel Chemla, Gennaro Chierchia, Jon Gajewski, Diane Lillo-Martin, William Snyder, Benjamin Spector, and the audience at the Workshop on Negation and Polarity: Interfaces and Cognition. I also thank Pierre Larrivée and three anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper. The research leading to these results has received funding from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP/2007-2013)/ERC Grant Agreement n.313610, and was supported by ANR-10-IDEX-0001-02 PSL* and ANR-10-LABX-0087 IEC.

References

  1. Arregui, A. (2008). Some remarks on domain widening. In N. Abner & J. Bishop (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 45–53).Google Scholar
  2. Bartsch, R. (1973). ‘Negative transportation’ gibt es nicht. Linguistische Berichte 27, 1–7.Google Scholar
  3. Chierchia, G. (2006). Broaden your views: Implicatures of domain widening and the “logicality”of language. Linguistic Inquiry, 37(4), 535–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Chierchia, G. (2013). Logic and grammar: Polarity, free choice, and intervention. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Crain, S., & McKee, C. (1985). The acquisition of structural restrictions on anaphora. NELS, 16 94–111.Google Scholar
  6. Crain, S., & Thornton, R. (1998). Investigations in universal grammar: A guide to experiments on the acquisition of syntax and semantics. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  7. Duffley, P. J., & Larrivée, P. (2010). Anyone for non-scalarity? English Language and Linguistics, 14(1), 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Fauconnier, G. (1975). Polarity and the scale principle. CLS, 11, 188–199.Google Scholar
  9. Fauconnier, G. (1979). Implication reversal in a natural language. In F. Guenther & S. J. Schmidt (Eds.), Formal semantics and pragmatics for natural languages, (pp. 289–301). Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
  10. Gajewski, J. R. (2007). Neg-raising and polarity. Linguistics and Philosophy, 30, 289–328.Google Scholar
  11. Giannakidou, A. (1998). Polarity sensitivity as (non)veridical dependency. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Giannakidou, A. (2011). Positive polarity items and negative polarity items: Variation, licensing, and compositionality. In C. Maienborn, et al. (Eds.), Semantics: An international handbook of natural language meaning, (Vol. 2, pp. 1660–1712). Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  13. Gualmini, A., & Crain, S. (2002). Why no child or adult must learn DeMorgan’s laws. In S. Catherine Howell, S. A. Fish & T. Keith-Lucas (Eds.), Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 367–378). Somerville: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
  14. Gualmini, A. (2004). Some knowledge children don’t lack. Linguistics, 42(5), 957–982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Horn, L. R. (1989). A natural history of negation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  16. Horn, L. R. (2016, this volume). Licensing NPIs: Some negative (and positive) results. In P. Larrivée & C. Lee (Eds.), Negation and polarity: Experimental perspectives (pp. 281–305). Cham: Springer.Google Scholar
  17. Kadmon, N., & Landman, F. (1993). Any. Linguistics and Philosophy, 16, 353–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Krifka, M. (1995). The semantics and pragmatics of polarity items. Linguistic Analysis, 25, 209–257.Google Scholar
  19. Kuczaj, S. (1977). The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 589–600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Ladusaw, W. (1979). Polarity sensitivity as inherent scope relations. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.Google Scholar
  21. Lee, R. K. (1993). NPI licensing and the domain of A-movement. Ms., University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
  22. Lin, J., Weerman, F., & Zeijlstra, H. (2013). Emerging NPIs. Paper presented at the 6th Semantics and Philosophy in Europe colloquium, Saint-Petersburg State University.Google Scholar
  23. Linebarger, M. (1987). Negative polarity and grammatical representation. Linguistics and Philosophy, 10(3), 325–387.Google Scholar
  24. MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed., Vol. 2). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  25. Nicolae, A. C. (2013). Any questions? Polarity as a window into the structure of questions. Ph.D. dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
  26. O’Leary, C., & Crain, S. (1994). Negative polarity items (a positive results), positive polarity items (a negative result). Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston University.Google Scholar
  27. Rowland, C., & Fletcher, S. L. (2006). The effect of sampling on estimates of lexical specificity and error rates. Journal of Child Language, 33, 859–877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Thornton, R. (1994). Children’s negative questions: A production/comprehension asymmetry. Paper presented at ESCOL, September, Columbia.Google Scholar
  29. Tieu, L. (2010a). On the tri-ambiguous status of ‘any’: The view from child language. In D. Lutz. & N. Li (Eds.), Proceedings of the 20 th Semantics and Linguistic Theory Conference (pp. 19–37).Google Scholar
  30. Tieu, L. (2010b). On the tri-ambiguous status of ‘any’: The view from child language. Paper presented at the 4th Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition in North America (GALANA), September 1st, University of Toronto.Google Scholar
  31. Tieu, L. (2013). Logic and grammar in child language: How children acquire the semantics of polarity sensitivity. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
  32. Tieu, L., & Lidz, J. (2014a). On the nature of any’s alternatives: Experimental evidence for domain widening. Ms., University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
  33. Tieu, L., Romoli, J., Zhou, P., & Crain, S. (2015). Children’s knowledge of free choice inferences and scalar implicatures. Journal of Semantics.Google Scholar
  34. van der Wal, S. (1996). Negative polarity items and negation: Tandem Acquisition. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Groningen.Google Scholar
  35. von Fintel, K. (1999). NPI licensing, Strawson entailment, and context dependency. Journal of Semantics, 16(2), 97–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Warren-Leubecker, A. (1982). Sex differences in speech to children. Ph.D. dissertation, Georgia Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
  37. Warren-Leubecker, A., & Bohannon, J. N. (1984). Intonation patterns in child-directed speech: Mother-father speech. Child Development, 55, 1379–1385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Xiang, M., Conroy, A., Lidz, J., & Zukowski, A. (2006). Children’s understanding of polarity items. Poster presented at Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing 2006, Nijmegen.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et PsycholinguistiqueCNRS, ENSParisFrance

Personalised recommendations