Advertisement

Self-Organisation of Conceptual Spaces from Quality Dimensions

  • Paul Vogt
Part of the Synthese Library book series (SYLI, volume 359)

Abstract

This chapter presents a discussion on how conceptual spaces can evolve from a set of quality dimensions, and how these spaces can become shared among a population of cognitive agents. An agent-based simulation of Steels’ Talking Heads experiment is presented in which virtual agents construct novel concepts, as well as a shared, simplified language from scratch. Simulations demonstrate that the structure of a conceptual space (i.e. from what quality dimensions it is composed) can evolve in a population of communicating agents. It is argued that the underlying mechanisms involve the following factors: the environment of the agents, their embodiment and cognitive capacities, self-organisation, and cultural transmission.

Keywords

Quality Dimension Cultural Evolution Conceptual Space Cultural Transmission Rule Type 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgements

The writing of this chapter was funded through a Vidi grant awarded by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO, grant no. 276-70-018). I wish to thank Frank Zenker, Peter Gärdenfors and all participants of the Conceptual Spaces at Work symposium for their valuable contributions in discussing this research. Also, many thanks to Emiel Krahmer and an anonymous reviewer for their valuable comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

References

  1. Arbib, M. A., & Bickerton, D. (Eds.). (2010). The emergence of protolanguage: Holophrasis vs compositionality. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  2. Baker, M. C. (2003). Linguistic differences and language design. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(8), 349–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baronchelli, A., Felici, M., Caglioti, E., Loreto, V., & Steels, L. (2006). Sharp transition towards shared lexicon in multi-agent systems. Journal of Statistical Mechanics, P06014.Google Scholar
  4. Batali, J. (2002). The negotiation and acquisition of recursive grammars as a result of competition among exemplars. In T. Briscoe (Ed.), Linguistic evolution through language acquisition: Formal and computational models (chap. 5). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 173–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bod, R., Sima’an, K., & Scha, R. (Eds.). (2003). Data oriented parsing. Stanford: Center for Study of Language and Information (CSLI) Publications.Google Scholar
  7. Bowerman, M., & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.). (2001). Language acquisition and conceptual development. Cambridge: Cambrige University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (2005). The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(6), 1482.Google Scholar
  10. Brighton, H., & Kirby, S. (2001). The survival of the smallest: Stability conditions for the cultural evolution of compositional language. In J. Kelemen & P. Sosík (Eds.), Proceeding of the 6th European conference on artificial life, ECAL 2001, Prague.Google Scholar
  11. Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Croft, W. (2000). Explaining language change: An evolutionary approach. New York: Longman.Google Scholar
  13. Croft, W. (2002). The Darwinization of linguistics. Selection, 3, 75–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Darwin, C. (1968). The origin of species. Penguin Books.Google Scholar
  15. Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. De Beule, J., & Bergen, B. K. (2006). On the emergence of compositionality. In A. Cangelosi, A. Smith, & K. Smith (Eds.), The evolution of language: Proceedings of the 6th international conference on the evolution of language, Rome.Google Scholar
  17. de Boer, B., & Vogt, P. (1999). Emergence of speech sounds in changing populations. In D. Floreano, J.-D. Nicoud, & F. Mondada (Eds.), Advances in artificial life: Proceedings of 5th European conference ECAL’99, Lausanne.Google Scholar
  18. de Greeff, J., & Belpaeme, T. (2011). The development of shared meaning within different embodiments. In 2011 IEEE international conference on development and learning (ICDL), Frankfurt am Main (Vol. 2, pp. 1–6).Google Scholar
  19. Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(05), 429–448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gärdenfors, P. (2000). Conceptual spaces. Cambridge: Bradford Books/MIT.Google Scholar
  21. Garrod, S., Fay, N., Rogers, S., Walker, B., & Swoboda, N. (2010). Can iterated learning explain the emergence of graphical symbols? Interaction Studies, 11(1), 33–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Haun, D., Rapold, C. J., Janzen, G., & Levinson, S. C. (2011). Plasticity of human spatial cognition: Spatial language and cognition covary across cultures. Cognition, 119(1), 70–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of language. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kalish, M. L., Griffiths, T. L., & Lewandowsky, S. (2007). Iterated learning: Intergenerational knowledge transmission reveals inductive biases. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(2), 288–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kirby, S. (2001). Spontaneous evolution of linguistic structure: An iterated learning model of the emergence of regularity and irregularity. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation, 5(2), 102–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kirby, S., & Hurford, J. R. (2002). The emergence of linguistic structure: An overview of the iterated learning model. In A. Cangelosi & D. Parisi (Eds.), Simulating the evolution of language (pp. 121–148). London: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kirby, S., Smith, K., & Brighton, H. (2004). From UG to universals: Linguistic adaptation through iterated learning. Studies in Language, 28(3), 587–607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(31), 10681–10686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Koolen, R., Gatt, A., Goudbeek, M., & Krahmer, E. (2011). Factors causing overspecification in definite descriptions. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(13), 3231–3250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Majid, A., Bowerman, M., Kita, S., Haun, D., & Levinson, S. C. (2004). Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(3), 108–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Mufwene, S. S. (2001). The ecology of language evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selecion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 707–789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Prigogine, I., & Strengers, I. (1984). Order out of chaos. New York: Bantam Books.Google Scholar
  34. Steels, L. (1996). Perceptually grounded meaning creation. In M. Tokoro (Ed.), Proceedings of the international conference on multi-agent systems, Kyoto. Menlo Park: AAAI Press.Google Scholar
  35. Steels, L. (1997). Synthesising the origins of language and meaning using coevolution, self-organisation and level formation. In J. Hurford, C. Knight, & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Steels, L. (2003). Evolving grounded communication for robots. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 308–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Steels, L. (Ed.). (2012). Experiments in cultural language evolution (Vol. 3). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  38. Steels, L., & Belpaeme, T. (2005). Coordinating perceptually grounded categories through language: A case study for colour. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 469–529.Google Scholar
  39. Steels, L., & Beule, J. D. (2006). Unify and merge in fluid construction grammar. In P. Vogt, Y. Sugita, E. Tuci, & C. Nehaniv (Eds.), Symbol grounding and beyond: Proceedings of the third international workshop on the emergence and evolution of linguistic communication, Rome (pp. 197–223). Springer.Google Scholar
  40. Steels, L., & Kaplan, F. (1998). Stochasticity as a source of innovation in language games. In Proceedings of alive VI.Google Scholar
  41. Steels, L., Kaplan, F., McIntyre, A., & Van Looveren, J. (2002). Crucial factors in the origins of word-meaning. In A. Wray (Ed.), The transition to language (pp. 252–271). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  42. van Zaanen, M. (2000). ABL: Alignment-based learning. In Proceedings of the 18th international conference on computational linguistics (COLING), Saarbrücken.Google Scholar
  43. Vogt, P. (2004). Minimum cost and the emergence of the Zipf-Mandelbrot law. In J. Pollack, M. Bedau, P. Husbands, T. Ikegami, & R. A. Watson (Eds.), Artificial life IX proceedings of the ninth international conference on the simulation and synthesis of living systems, Boston (pp. 214–219). MIT.Google Scholar
  44. Vogt, P. (2005a). The emergence of compositional structures in perceptually grounded language games. Artificial Intelligence, 167(1–2), 206–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Vogt, P. (2005b). Meaning development versus predefined meanings in language evolution models. In L. Kaelbling & A. Saffiotti (Eds.), Proceedings of IJCAI-05, Edinburgh (pp. 1154–1159). IJCAI.Google Scholar
  46. Vogt, P. (2005c). On the acquisition and evolution of compositional languages: Sparse input and the productive creativity of children. Adaptive Behavior, 13(4), 325–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Vogt, P. (2006a). Cumulative cultural evolution: Can we ever learn more? In S. Nolfi et al. (Eds.), From animals to animats 9: Proceedings of the ninth international conference on simulation of adaptive behaviour, Rome. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  48. Vogt, P. (2006b). Overextensions and the emergence of compositionality. In A. Cangelosi, A. Smith, & K. Smith (Eds.), The evolution of language: Proceedings of the 6th international conference on the evolution of language, Rome. World Scientific Press.Google Scholar
  49. Vogt, P. (2007). Variation, competition and selection in the self-organisation of compositionality. In B. Wallace, A. Ross, J. B. Davies, & T. Anderson (Eds.), The mind, the body and the world: Psychology after cognitivism? (pp. 233–256). Exeter: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
  50. Vogt, P., & Mastin, J. D. (2013). Anchoring social symbol grounding in children’s interactions. Künstliche Intelligenz, 27, 145–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wellens, P., Loetzsch, M., & Steels, L. (2008). Flexible word meaning in embodied agents. Connection Science, 20(2), 173–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  53. Wray, A. (1998). Protolanguage as a holistic systemfor social interaction. Language and Communication, 18, 47–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Tilburg centre for Cognition and CommunicationTilburg UniversityTilburgThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations