Advertisement

From Conceptual Spaces to Predicates

  • Jean-Louis Dessalles
Part of the Synthese Library book series (SYLI, volume 359)

Abstract

Why is a red face not really red? How do we decide that this book is a textbook or not? Conceptual spaces provide the medium on which these computations are performed, but an additional operation is needed: Contrast. By contrasting a reddish face with a prototypical face, one gets a prototypical ‘red’. By contrasting this book with a prototypical textbook, the lack of exercises may pop out. Dynamic contrasting is an essential operation for converting perceptions into predicates. The existence of dynamic contrasting may contribute to explaining why lexical meanings correspond to convex regions of conceptual spaces. But it also explains why predication is most of the time opportunistic, depending on context. While off-line conceptual similarity is a holistic operation, the contrast operation provides a context-dependent distance that creates ephemeral predicative judgments (‘this book is not a textbook’, ‘this author is a linguist’) that are essential for interfacing conceptual spaces with natural language and with reasoning.

Keywords

Conceptual spaces Contrast Predication Language of thought Aspect 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research is based on past collaborations with Laleh Ghadakpour and with Damien Munch. I would like to thank Damien Munch for his fruitful comments. Part of this research is funded by the “Chaire Modélisation des Imaginaires, Innovation et Création” (http://imaginaires.telecom-paristech.fr).

References

  1. Bickerton, D. (1990). Language and species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  2. Bratman, M. E., Israel, D. J., & Pollack, M. E. (1988). Plans and resource-bounded practical reasoning. Computational Intelligence, 4(4), 349–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Chomsky, N. (2000). New horizons in the study of language and mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Dessalles, J.-L. (1998). Altruism, status, and the origin of relevance. In J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy, & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language: Social and cognitive bases (pp. 130–147). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Dessalles, J.-L. (2007). Why we talk – The evolutionary origins of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Dessalles, J.-L. (2008). La pertinence et ses origines cognitives – Nouvelles théories. Paris: Hermes-Science Publications.Google Scholar
  7. Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Oxford: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Fodor, J. A. (1981). Representations: Philosophical essays on the foundations of cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  9. Fodor, J. A. (1994). Concepts: A potboiler. Cognition, 50, 95–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fodor, J. A. (1998). Concepts: Where cognitive science went wrong. Oxford: Clarendon.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Fodor, J. A., & Lepore, E. (1992). Holism – A shoper’s guide. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  12. Fodor, J. A., Garrett, M. F., Walker, E. C. T., & Parkes, C. H. (1980). Against definitions. Cognition, 8, 263–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gärdenfors, P. (2000). Conceptual spaces: The geometry of thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  14. Gärdenfors, P. (2014). Geometry of meaning – Semantics based on conceptual spaces. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  15. Ghadakpour, L. (2003). Le système conceptuel, à l’interface entre le langage, le raisonnement et l’espace qualitatif: vers un modèle de représentations éphémères. Thèse de doctorat, Ecole Polytechnique, Paris.Google Scholar
  16. Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42, 335–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hurford, J. R. (2003). The neural basis of predicate-argument structure. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(3), 261–283.Google Scholar
  18. Jackendoff, R. (1983). Semantics and cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  19. Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1977). Procedural semantics. Cognition, 5, 189–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kamp, H., & Partee, B. (1995). Prototype theory and compositionality. Cognition, 57(2), 129–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Machery, E. (2009). Doing without concepts. Cambridge, MA: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Munch, D. (2013). Un modèle dynamique et parcimonieux du traitement automatisé de l’aspect dans les langues naturelles. PhD dissertation, to appear Telecom ParisTech 2013-ENST-0058.Google Scholar
  24. Munch, D., & Dessalles, J.-L. (2014). Assessing parsimony in models of aspect. In P. Bello, M. Guarini, M. McShane, & B. Scassellati (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2121–2126). Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  25. Murphy, M. L. (2003). Semantic relations and the lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Paradis, C., & Willners, C. (2011). Antonymy: From convention to meaning-making. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 9(2), 367–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Pepperberg, I. M. (1999). The Alex studies – Cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrots. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, ed. 2000.Google Scholar
  28. Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  29. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., & Lewin, R. (1994). Kanzi: The ape at the brink of the human mind. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  30. Talmy, L. (1988). Force dynamics in language and thought. Cognitive Science, 12(1), 49–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Vendler, Z. (1967). Linguistics in philosophy. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Network and Computer Science DepartmentTelecom ParisTechParisFrance

Personalised recommendations