Advertisement

What Is Knowledge? Who Creates It? Who Possesses It? The Need for Novel Answers to Old Questions

  • Aileen OeberstEmail author
  • Joachim Kimmerle
  • Ulrike Cress
Chapter
Part of the Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning Series book series (CULS, volume 16)

Abstract

This chapter provides an overview of both traditional and more recent conceptualizations of knowledge. It also aims at identifying shortcomings of each of these conceptualizations in the context of mass collaboration. Philosophy has traditionally conceptualized knowledge as “justified true belief,” whereas psychology tends to consider it mainly in terms of semantic memory. What both traditions have in common, however, is an individualistic focus, which emphasizes that knowledge is located within people’s minds. But this individual perspective reaches its limits when considering transpersonal phenomena like collaborative knowledge construction and mass collaboration. More recent approaches have taken social aspects of knowledge-related processes into account. We present some of them briefly and discuss them with regard to mass collaboration settings. Finally, we present a systemic view of knowledge as an attempt toward a synthesis of individualistic and social perspectives, which might be more suitable for mass collaboration scenarios.

Keywords

Collaboration Mass collaboration Knowledge Social conceptualizations of knowledge Systemic view of knowledge 

References

  1. Adler, J. (2010). Epistemological problems of testimony. In Edward N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Winter 2010 ed.). Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/testimony-episprob/
  2. Alkin, M. C. (1992). Encyclopedia of educational research (6th ed.). New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  3. Alston, W. P. (1988). The deontological conception of epistemic justification. Philosophical Perspectives, 2, 257–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ballard, P. B. (1913). Oblivescence and reminiscence. British Journal of Psychology, 1(No. 2, Monograph Supplements), Preface-82.Google Scholar
  5. Bar-Tal, D., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1988). The social psychology of knowledge. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Bereiter, C. & Scardamalia, M. (2010). Can children really create knowledge? Canadian Journal of Learning and Technology, 36(1). Retrieved from http://www.cjlt.ca/index.php/cjlt/article/view/585/289
  8. Boghossian, P. A. (2006). Fear of knowledge: Against relativism and constructivism. Oxford, England: Clarendon.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Caramazza, A., McCloskey, M., & Green, B. (1981). Naïve beliefs in “sophisticated” subjects: Misconceptions about the trajectories of objects. Cognition, 9, 117–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cress, U., Feinkohl, I., Jirschitzka, J., & Kimmerle, J. (2016). Mass collaboration as co-evolution of cognitive and social systems. In U. Cress, J. Moskaliuk, & H. Jeong (Eds.), Mass collaboration and education. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.Google Scholar
  11. Cress, U., & Kimmerle, J. (2008). A systemic and cognitive view on collaborative knowledge building with wikis. International Journal of Computer Supported Collaborative Learning, 3, 105–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Damşa, C. I., Kirschner, P. A., Andriessen, J. E. B., Erkens, G., & Sins, P. H. M. (2010). Shared epistemic agency: An empirical study of an emergent construct. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 19, 143–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. David, M. (2013). The correspondence theory of truth. In Edward N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2013 ed.). Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/truth-correspondence/
  14. Dienes, Z., & Perner, J. (1999). A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 735–808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dunn, J. C. (2004). Remember—know: A matter of confidence. Psychological Review, 2004(111), 524–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Ebbinghaus, H. (1964). Memory (H. A. Ruger & C. E. Bussenius, Trans.). New York, NY: Dover (Original work published 1885).Google Scholar
  17. Erdelyi, M. H. (2010). The ups and downs of memory. American Psychologist, 65, 623–633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Faulkner, P. (2006). Understanding knowledge transmission. Ratio, 19, 156–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Feldman, R., & Conee, E. (1985). Evidentialism. Philosophical Studies, 48, 15–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fiedler, K., Russer, S., & Gramm, K. (1993). Illusory correlations and memory performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 111–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fleck, L. (1935). Entstehung und Entwicklung einer wissenschaftlichen Tatsache. Einführung in die Lehre vom Denkstil und Denkkollektiv [The genesis and development of a scientific fact]. Basel, Switzerland: Schwabe.Google Scholar
  22. Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Functional aspects of recollective experience. Memory & Cognition, 16, 309–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gettier, E. L. (1963). Is justified true belief knowledge? Analysis, 23, 121–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Giere, R. N., & Moffatt, B. (2003). Distributed cognition: Where the cognitive and the social merge. Social Studies of the Science, 33, 301–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gigerenzer, G. (2004). Fast and frugal heuristics: The tools of bounded rationality. In D. J. Koehler & N. Harvey (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 62–88). Malden, MA: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahnemann, D. (2002). Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge, England: University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Goldman, A. (1979). What is justified belief? In G. Pappas (Ed.), Justification and knowledge. Boston, MA: D. Reidel.Google Scholar
  28. Goldman, A. (1987). Foundations of social epistemics. Synthese, 73, 109–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Goldman, A. (2010). Systems-oriented social epistemology. In T. Gendler & J. Hawthorne (Eds.), Oxford studies in epistemology (vol. 3). Retrieved from http://fas-philosophy.rutgers.edu/goldman/Systems-Oriented%20Social%20Epistemology.pdf
  30. Goldman, A., & Blanchard, T. (2010). Social epistemology. In Edward N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Summer 2010 ed.). Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/epistemology-social/
  31. Greco, J. & Turri, J. (2011). Virtue epistemology. In Edward N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Spring 2011 ed.). Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/epistemology-virtue/
  32. Greenwald, A. G. (1975). Consequences of prejudice against the null hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Griffith, A. K., & Preston, K. R. (1992). Grade-12 students’ misconceptions relating to fundamental characteristics of atoms and molecules. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 29, 611–628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Gruber, T. (2011). Gedächtnis—Lehrbuch Basiswissen Psychologie. Wiesbaden, Germany: VS-Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften.Google Scholar
  35. Hardwig, J. (1985). Epistemic dependence. The Journal of Philosophy, 82, 335–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Higgins, E. T., & Rholes, W. S. (1978). “Saying is believing”: Effects of message modification on memory and liking for the person described. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 363–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hinsz, V. B., Tindale, R. S., & Vollrath, D. A. (1997). The emerging conceptualization of groups as information processors. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 43–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Hirst, W., & Manier, D. (2008). Towards a psychology of collective memory. Memory, 16, 183–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ichikawa, J. J. & Steup, M. (2014). The analysis of knowledge. In Edward N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Spring 2014 ed.). Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/knowledge-analysis/
  40. Kerr, N. L., MacCoun, R. J., & Kramer, G. P. (1996). Biases in judgment: Comparing individuals and groups. Psychological Review, 103, 687–719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kimmerle, J., Cress, U., & Held, C. (2010). The interplay between individual and collective knowledge: Technologies for organisational learning and knowledge building. Knowledge Management Research & Practice, 8, 33–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kimmerle, J., Gerbing, K.-K., Cress, U., & Thiel, A. (2012). Exchange of complementary and alternative medical knowledge in sport-related Internet fora. Sociology of Sport Journal, 29, 348–364.Google Scholar
  43. Kimmerle, J., Moskaliuk, J., Cress, U., & Thiel, A. (2011). A systems theoretical approach to online knowledge building. AI & Society: Journal of Knowledge, Culture and Communication, 26, 49–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kimmerle, J., Moskaliuk, J., Harrer, A., & Cress, U. (2010). Visualizing co-evolution of individual and collective knowledge. Information, Communication and Society, 13, 1099–1121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Kimmerle, J., Moskaliuk, J., Oeberst, A., & Cress, U. (2015). Learning and collective knowledge construction with social media: A process-oriented perspective. Educational Psychologist, 50, 120–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Kitcher, P. (1990). The division of cognitive labor. The Journal of Philosophy, 87, 5–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Klemke, E. D. (1979). Karl Popper, objective knowledge, and the third world. Philosophia, 9, 45–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Klimoski, R., & Mohammed, S. (1994). Team mental model: Construct or metaphor? Journal of Management, 20, 403–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Knorr-Cetina, K. (1981). The manufacture of knowledge: An essay on the constructivist and contextual nature of science. New York, NY: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  50. Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  51. Kusch, M. (2002a). Testimony in communitarian epistemology. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 33, 335–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Kusch, M. (2002b). Knowledge by agreement: The programme of communitarian epistemology. Oxford, England: University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Lakatos, I. (1970). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth in knowledge. London, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Larson, J. R., Jr., & Christensen, C. (1993). Groups as problem-solving units: Toward a new meaning of social cognition. British Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 5–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Lehrer, K. (1987). Personal and social knowledge. Synthese, 73, 87–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Lindner, I., Echterhoff, G., Davidson, P. S. R., & Brand, M. (2010). Observation inflation: Your actions become mine. Psychological Science, 21, 1291–1299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Luhmann, N. (1984). Soziale Systeme. Grundriß einer allgemeinen Theorie [Social systems. Outline of a general theory]. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar
  58. Luhmann, N. (1990). Die Wissenschaft der Gesellschaft [Science as a social system]. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar
  59. Maier, J., & Richter, T. (2014). Verstehen multipler Texte zu kontroversen wissenschaftlichen Themen: Die Rolle der epistemischen Validierung. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 42, 24–38.Google Scholar
  60. Mandl, H. & Spada, H. (1988). Wissenspsychologie [The Psychology of Knowledge]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
  61. Matthiessen, H. O., & Willaschek, M. (2009). Epistemische Rechtfertigung. In H. J. Sandkühler (Ed.), Enzyklopädie Philosophie (3rd ed.). Hamburg, Germany: Meiner.Google Scholar
  62. Maturana, H. R., & Varela, F. J. (1987). The tree of knowledge: The biological roots of human understanding. Boston, MA: New Science Library/Shambhala Publications.Google Scholar
  63. Moskaliuk, J., Kimmerle, J., & Cress, U. (2009). Wiki-supported learning and knowledge building: Effects of incongruity between knowledge and information. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 25, 549–561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Moussavi, F., & Evans, D. A. (1993). Emergence of organizational attributions: The role of a shared cognitive schema. Journal of Management, 19, 79–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Oeberst, A. (2012). If anything else comes to mind… better keep it to yourself? Delayed recall is discrediting—unjustifiably. Law and Human Behavior, 36, 266–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Oeberst, A., & Blank, H. (2012). Undoing suggestive influence on memory: The reversibility of the misinformation effect. Cognition, 125, 141–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Oeberst, A., Halatchliyski, I., Kimmerle, J., & Cress, U. (2014). Knowledge construction in Wikipedia: A systemic-constructivist analysis. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 23, 149–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Paalova, S., & Hakkarainen, K. (2005). The knowledge creation metaphor—An emergent epistemological approach to learning. Science & Education, 14, 535–557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Pappas, G. (2009). Internalist vs. externalist conceptions of epistemic justification. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Spring 2009 ed.). Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/justep-intext/
  70. Peressini, A. (2008). Proof, reliability, and mathematical knowledge. Theoria, 69, 211–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 123–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Piaget, J. (1977). The development of thought: Equilibration of cognitive structures. New York, NY: Viking.Google Scholar
  73. Pohl, R. F. (2004). Cognitive illusions: A handbook on fallacies and biases in thinking, judgement and memory. Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  74. Popper, K. R. (1968). Epistemology without a knowing subject. Studies in Logic and the Foundations of Mathematics, 52, 333–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Popper, K. R. (1970). Die offene Gesellschaft und ihre Feinde [The open society and its enemies]. Bern, Switzerland: Francke.Google Scholar
  76. Popper, K. R. (1972). Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Oxford, England: Clarendon.Google Scholar
  77. Popper, K. R. (1978). Three worlds: The Tanner lecture on human values. Lecture delivered at the University of Michigan, April 7, 1978. Retrieved from http://tannerlectures.utah.edu/_documents/a-to-z/p/popper80.pdf
  78. Putnam, H. (1975). The meaning of “meaning”. Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, 7, 131–193.Google Scholar
  79. Reber, R., & Unckelbach, C. (2010). The epistemic status of processing fluency as source for judgments of truth. Review of Philosophical Psychology, 1, 563–581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Resnick, L. B., Levine, J. M., & Teasley, S. D. (Eds.). (1991). Perspectives on socially shared cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  81. Richter, T. (2003). Epistemologische Einschätzungen beim Textverstehen [Epistemological evaluations in text understanding]. Lengerich, Germany: Pabst.Google Scholar
  82. Russell, B. (1910). Knowledge by acquaintance and knowledge by description. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 11, 108–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Salomon, G. (Ed.). (1993). Distributed cognitions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  84. Scardamalia, M. & Bereiter, C. (2010). A brief history of knowledge building. Canadian Journal of Learning and Technology, 36(1). Retrieved from http://www.cjlt.ca/index.php/cjlt/article/view/574/276
  85. Schiffer, S. (1996). Contextualist solutions to skepticism. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, New Series, 96, 317–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Schroeder, S., Richter, T., & Hoever, I. (2008). Getting a picture that is both accurate and stable: Situation models and epistemic validation. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 237–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Shaw, J., & Porter, S. (2015). Constructing rich false memories of committing crime. Psychological Science. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1177/0956797614562862
  88. Sperling, M., & Schmidt, T. (2009). Allgemeine Psychologie kompakt—Wahrnehmung, Aufmerksamkeit, Sprache, Denken. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
  89. Sporer, S. L., Penrod, S. D., Read, J. D., & Cutler, B. L. (1995). Choosing, confidence, and accuracy: A meta-analysis of the confidence-accuracy relation in eyewitness identification studies. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 315–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Steck, P., Hermanutz, M., Lafrenz, B., Schwind, D., Hettler, S., Maier, B., & Geiger, S. (2010). Die psychometrische Qualität von Realkennzeichen [Psychometric characteristics of the criteria based content analysis]. Retrieved from http://serwiss.bib.hs-hannover.de/frontdoor/index/index/docId/263
  91. Steffens, M. C., & Mecklenbräuker, S. (2007). False memories. Phenomena, theories, and implications. Journal of Psychology, 215, 12–24.Google Scholar
  92. Thompson, L., & Fine, G. A. (1999). Socially shared cognition, affect, and behavior: A review and integration. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 278–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Tulving, E. (1985a). How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist, 40, 385–398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Tulving, E. (1985b). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 25, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Tulving, E. (1987). Multiple memory systems and consciousness. Human Neurobiology, 6, 67–80.Google Scholar
  96. Tulving, E. (1989). Memory: Performance, knowledge, and experience. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 1, 3–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Vahid, H. (1998). Deontic vs. nondeontic conceptions of epistemic justification. Erkenntnis, 49, 285–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Vicente, K. J., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). Reconstructive remembering of the scientific literature. Cognition, 46, 101–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. von Foerster, H. (2003). Understanding understanding: Essays on cybernetics and cognition. New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  101. Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., & Raymond, P. (1991). Transactive memory in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 923–929.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Wilson, R. A., & Keil, F. C. (1999). The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  103. Womack, C. A. (1993). The crucial role of proof: A classical defense against mathematical empiricism. Doctoral Dissertation at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Retrieved from http://dspace.mit.edu/handle/1721.1/12678
  104. Wood, J. M., & Nezworski, M. T. (2005). Science as a history of corrected mistakes. American Psychologist, 60, 657–658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Aileen Oeberst
    • 1
    Email author
  • Joachim Kimmerle
    • 1
  • Ulrike Cress
    • 1
  1. 1.Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien, Knowledge Construction LabTuebingenGermany

Personalised recommendations