Skip to main content

Prosody and Intention Recognition

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Explicit and Implicit Prosody in Sentence Processing

Abstract

Listeners face multiple challenges in mapping prosody onto intentions: The relevant intentions vary with the general context of an utterance (e.g., the speaker’s goals) and how prosodic contours are realized varies across speakers, accents, and speech conditions. We propose that listeners map acoustic information onto prosodic representations using (rational) probabilistic inference, in the form of generative models, which are updated on the fly based on the match between predictions and the input. We review some ongoing work, motivated by this framework, focusing on the “It looks like an X” construction, which, depending on the pitch contour and context, can be interpreted as “It looks like an X and it is” or “It looks like an X and it isn’t.” We use this construction to investigate the hypothesis that pragmatic processing shows the pattern of adaptation effects that is expected if the mapping of speech onto intentions involves rational inference.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    A preliminary report of these three studies, including the methodological details and results, is presented in Kurumada et al. (2012). A longer manuscript reporting these results is under review (Kurumada et al. n.d.).

References

  • Beckman, M. E., & Ayers, G. M. (1994). Guidelines for ToBI labeling. http://www.ling.ohio-state.edu/research/phonetics/E ToBI. Accessed 23 Sept 2008.

  • Beckman, M. E., & Hirschberg, J. (1994). The ToBI annotiation conventions. http://www.ling.ohio-state.edu/˜tobi/ame tobi/annotation conventions.html. Accessed 23 Sept 2008.

  • Bibyk, S., Kurumada, C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (n.d.). Context constraints on intonation interpretation (in preparation).

    Google Scholar 

  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2008). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (version 5.0.26) [computer program]. http://www.praat.org/. Accessed 16 June 2008.

  • Brown, M., Dilley, L. C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2015). Real-time expectations based on context speech rate can cause words to appear or disappear. In Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1374–1379).

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, M., Salverda, A. P., Gunlogson, C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2015). Interpreting prosodic cues in discourse context. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 149–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113(2), 234–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1992). Arenas of language use. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clayards, M., Tanenhaus, M. K., Aslin, R. N., & Jacobs, R. A. (2008). Perception of speech reflects optimal use of probabilistic speech cues. Cognition, 108(3), 804–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connine, C. M., & Clifton, C., Jr. (1987). Interactive use of lexical information in speech perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance, 13(2), 291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. M. (1974). Control of eye fixation by meaning of spoken language: New methodology for real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Degen, J. (2013). Alternatives in pragmatic reasoning. PhD dissertation, University of Rochester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G., & Chang, F. (2013). The P-chain: Relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennison, H. Y., & Schafer, A. (2010). Online construction of implicature through contrastive prosody. Proceedings of 5th Speech Prosody Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L., & McClelland, J. L. (1988). Cognitive penetration of the mechanisms of perception: Compensation for coarticulation of lexically restored phonemes. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 143–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, T., Brown, M., & Tanenhaus, M. (2013). Prediction, explanation, and the role of generative models in language processing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 211–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 10(3), 279–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fine, A. B., & Jaeger, T. F. (2013). Evidence for implicit learning in syntactic comprehension. Cognitive Science, 37(3), 578–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fine, A. B., Jaeger, T. F., Farmer, T., & Qian, T. (2013). Rapid expectation adaptation during syntactic comprehension. PLoS ONE, 8(10), e77661. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0077661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ganong, W. F. (1990). Phonetic categorization in auditory word perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 110–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. Syntax and Semantics, 3, 41–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanna, J., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Trueswell, J. C. (2003). The effects of common ground and perspective on domains of referential interpretation. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 43–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, M. B., & Markman, E. M. (2005). Appearance questions can be misleading: Adiscourse-based account of the appearance-reality problem. Cognitive Psychology, 50(3), 233–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heller, D., Grodner, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). The role of perspective in identifying domains of reference. Cognition, 108(3), 831–836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isaacs, A., & Watson, D. (2010). Accent detection is a slippery slope: Direction and rate of f0 change drives listeners comprehension. Language Cognitive Processes, 25(7), 1178–1200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, K., & Speer, S. R. (2008). Anticipatory effects of intonation: Eye movements during instructed visual search. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 541–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (1972). Semantics in generative grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger, T. F., & Snider, N. (2013). Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime’s prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition, 127(1), 57–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinschmidt, D., & Jaeger, T. F. (2011). A Bayesian belief updating model of phonetic recalibration and selective adaptation. In ACL workshop on cognitive modeling and computational linguistics. Portland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleinschmidt, D., & Jaeger, T. F. (2015). Robust speech perception: Recognizing the familiar, generalizing to the similar, and adapting to the novel. Psychological Review, 122(2), 148–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleinschmidt, D., Fine, A., & Jaeger, T. (2012). A belief-updating model of adaptation and cue combination in syntactic comprehension. Proceedings of the 34th annual conference of the cognitive science society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraljic, T., & Samuel, A. G. (2006). Generalization in perceptual learning for speech. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13(2), 262–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurumada, C. (2013). Navigating variability in the linguistic signal: Learning to interpret contrastive prosody. PhD dissertation, Stanford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurumada, C., Brown, M., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2012). Prosody and pragmatic inference: It looks like speech adaptation. Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurumada, C., Brown, M., Bibyk, S., Pontillo, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2013). Incremental processing in the pragmatic interpretation of contrastive prosody. Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurumada, C., Brown, M., Bibyk, S., Pontillo, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2014a). Is it or isn’t it: Listeners make rapid use of prosody to infer speaker meanings. Cognition, 133, 335–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurumada, C., Brown, M., Bibyk, S., Pontillo, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2014b). Rapid adaptation in online pragmatic interpretation of contrastive prosody. Proceedings of the 36th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurumada, C., Brown, M., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (n.d.). Probabilistic inferences in pragmatic interpretation of English contrastive prosody (submitted).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladd, D. R. (2008). Intonational phonology (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The trace model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McMurray, B., & Jongman, A. (2011). What information is necessary for speech categorization? Harnessing variability in the speech signal by integrating cues computed relative to expectations. Psychological Review, 118(2), 218–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. L., Green, K., & Schermer, T. (1984). A distinction between the effects of sentential speaking rate and semantic congruity on word identification. Perception & Psychophysics, 36, 329–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moulines, E., & Charpentier, F. (1990). Pitch-synchronous waveform processing techniques for text-to-speech synthesis using diphones. Speech Communication, 9(5–6), 453–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norris, D., McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (2003). Perceptual learning in speech. Cognitive Psychology, 47(2), 204–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierrehumbert, J. & Hirschberg, J. (1990). The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. In P. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions and plans in communication and discourse (pp. 271–311). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedivy, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., Chambers, C. G., & Carlson, G. N. (1999). Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through contextual representation. Cognition, 71, 109–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, K., Beckman, M., Pitrelli, J., Ostendorf, M., Wightman, C., Price, P., et al. (1992). ToBI: A standard for labeling English prosody. In International conference on spoken language processing (Vol. 2, pp. 867–870). Banff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K., & Sedivy, J. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, G., & Hirschberg, J. (1985). Implicating uncertainty: The pragmatics of fall-rise intonation. Language, 61, 747–776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Gunlogson, C., & Tanenhaus, M. (2008). Interpreting pitch accents in on-line comprehension: H* vs L + H*. Cognitive Science, 32, 1232–1244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, A., Braun, B., & Crocker, M. W. (2006). Finding referents in time: Eye-tracking evidence for the role of contrastive accents. Language and Speech, 49, 367–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yildirim, I., Degen, J., Tanenhaus, M. K. & Jaeger, T. F. (2013). Linguistic variability and adaptation in quantifier meanings. In Proceedings of the Thirty-Fifth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael K. Tanenhaus .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Tanenhaus, M., Kurumada, C., Brown, M. (2015). Prosody and Intention Recognition. In: Frazier, L., Gibson, E. (eds) Explicit and Implicit Prosody in Sentence Processing. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, vol 46. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-12961-7_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics