Skip to main content

Social Cognitive Development from an Evolutionary Perspective

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Evolutionary Perspectives on Social Psychology

Part of the book series: Evolutionary Psychology ((EVOLPSYCH))

Abstract

Humans are among the most social of mammals; they transmit information to group members and across generations with great fidelity. We contend, as others have (e.g., Alexander, The human revolution: behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans, Princeton University Press, Princeton, 1989, pp. 455–513; Dunbar, Annu Rev Anthropol 32:163–181, 2003; Hare, Annu Rev Anthropol 40:293–309, 2011), that it was changes in social cognition over hominin evolution that was the driving force in human intelligence. Moreover, the origins of humans’ social nature and cognition are found in infancy and childhood, placing social cognitive development at center stage in understanding the evolution of the human mind. Humans have a fierce bias for orienting toward social stimuli beginning very shortly after birth, as outlined by our discussion on newborns’ attention to social stimuli such as biological motion, facial expressions, and eye gaze. Beyond the neonatal period, we go on to discuss the development of infants and young children’s ability to view other people as intentional agents and perspective taking. Given that viewing others as intentional agents is a foundational ability for effective social learning, we outline how the nature of children’s social learning changes as they enter childhood by shifting their attention to the behavior of others and begin reproducing others’ actions, often based on their assumed intentions. Lastly, we contrast the review of the development of human social cognition during the early years of life with the social cognitive abilities of humans’ closest genetic relatives, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and speculate why such a unique cognitive system may have evolved.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abravanel, E., & Sigafoos, A. D. (1984). Exploring the presence of imitation during early infancy. Child Development, 55, 381–392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, R. D. (1989). Evolution of the human psyche. In P. Mellers & C. Stringer (Eds.), The human revolution: Behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans (pp. 455–513). Princeton: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human agency. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 164–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bard, K. A. (2007). Neonatal imitation in chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes) tested with two paradigms. Animal Cognition, 10, 233–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Batki, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Connellan, J., & Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Is there an innate gaze module? Evidence from human neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 223–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bardi, L., Regolin, L., & Simion, F. (2011). Biological motion preference in humans at birth: Role of dynamic and configural properties. Developmental Science, 14, 353–359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., & Kramer, S. J. (1987). Perception of biomechanical motions by infants: Implementation of various processing constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 577.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1987). How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children’s memory: An interpretive review. Developmental Review, 7, 93–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role of immaturity in human development. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 153–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (2003). Evolutionary developmental psychology: A new tool for better understanding human ontogeny. Human Development, 46, 259–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (2006). Mother knows best: Epigenetic inheritance, maternal effects, and the evolution of human intelligence. Developmental Review, 26, 213–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Bering, J. M. (2003). Big brains, slow development, and social complexity: The developmental and evolutionary origins of social cognition. In M. Brane, H. Ribbert, & W. Schiefernhvel (Eds.), The social brain: Evolution and Pathology (pp. 111–151). New York: Wiley.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Rosenberg, J. S. (2005). The role of developmental plasticity in the evolution of human cognition. In B. J. Ellis & D. F. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 45–75). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Sellers, P. D., II. (2013). Memory development in evolutionary perspective. In P. J. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory (pp. 126–150). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., Yunger, J. L., Bering, J. M., & Ragan, P. (2002). The generalization of deferred imitation in enculturated chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 5, 49–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., Cormier, C., & Rosenberg, J. S. (2005). The evolution of theory of mind: Big brains, social complexity, and inhibition. In W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability and theory of mind (pp. 147–174). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., Causey, K., & Periss, V. (2010). The evolution and development of human social cognition. In P. Kappeler & J. Silk (Eds.), Mind the gap: Tracing the origins of human universals (pp. 351–371). Berlin: Springer Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch-Achermann, H., & Boesch, C. (1993). Tool use in wild chimpanzees: New light from dark forests. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 18–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Chimpanzee and human cultures. Current Anthropology, 39, 591–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogin, B. (2001). The growth of humanity. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. Developmental Psychology, 38, 958–966.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bushnell, I. W. R., Sai, F., & Mullin, J. T. (1989). Neonatal recognition of the mother’s face. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 3–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Developmental Science, 10, F31–F38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W. (2005). Social cognition: Imitation, imitation, imitation. Current Biology, 15, R489–R500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1996). The effect of humans on the cognitive development of apes. In A. E. Russon, K. A. Bard, & S. T. Parker (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 371–403). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1995). Jeopardy in the courtroom: A scientific analysis of children’s testimony. Washington: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man. D. Appleton and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F. B. (1999). Cultural primatology comes of age. Nature, 399, 635–636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. (2003). The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 163–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 9602–9605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari, P. F., Visalberghi, E., Paukner, A., Fogassi, L., Ruggiero, A., & Suomi, S. J. (2006). Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLoS Biology, 4, e302.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2012). Experimental “microcultures” in young children: Identifying biographic, cognitive, and social predictors of information transmission. Child Development, 83, 911–925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gardiner, A. K. (2014). Beyond irrelevant actions: Understanding the role of intentionality in children’s imitation of relevant actions. Journal of experimental child psychology, 119, 54–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardiner, A. K., Greif, M. L., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2011). Guided by intention: preschoolers’ imitation reflects inferences of causation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 355–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gava, L., Valenza, E., Turati, C., & Schonen, S. D. (2008). Effect of partial occlusion on newborns’ face preference and recognition. Developmental Science, 11, 563–574.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755–755.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, E., McEwen, B. S., Dolan, L. M., Schafer-Kalkhoff, T., & Adler, N. E. (2005). Social disadvantage and adolescent stress. Journal of Adolescent Health, 37, 484–492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goren, C. C., Sarty, M., & Wu, P. Y. (1975). Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics, 56, 544–549.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavioral development: Theory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, R. D. (2011). From hominoid to hominid mind: What changes and why? Annual Review of Anthropology, 40, 293–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, M. (1989). Neonatal imitation, gaze aversion, and mother-infant interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 495–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hernández Blasi, C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2003). Evolutionary developmental psychology: A new tool for better understanding human ontogeny. Human Development, 46, 259–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 1360–1366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, L. M., Flynn, E. G., Wood, L. A., & Whiten, A. (2010). Observational learning of tool use in children: Investigating cultural spread through diffusion chains and learning mechanisms through ghost displays. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 82–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes) and children ( Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, S. W. (1979). Matching behavior in the young infant. Child Development, 50, 425–430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joffe, T. H. (1997). Social pressures have selected for an extended juvenile period in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 32, 593–605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H. (2007). The social brain in infancy: A developmental cognitive neuroscience approach. In D. Coch, K. W. Fischer, & G. Dawson (Eds.), Human behavior, learning, and the developing brain: Typical development (pp. 115–137). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1996). The connectionist infant: Would Piaget turn in his grave? Society for Research in Child Development, (Newsletter, Fall), 10, 1–3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawamura, S. (1959). The process of sub-culture propagation among Japanese macaques. Primates, 2, 43–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konner, M. (2010). Evolution of childhood: Relationships, emotion, mind. New York: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2006). 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 173–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science, 10, F1–F7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 19751–19756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGuigan, N., & Whiten, A. (2009). Emulation and “overemulation” in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23-and 30-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 367–381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGuigan, N., Makinson, J., & Whiten, A. (2011). From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 1–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1988). Imitation of televised models by infants. Child Development, 59, 1221–1229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mondloch, C. J., Lewis, T. L., Budreau, D. R., Maurer, D., Dannemiller, J. L., Stephens, B. R., & Kleiner-Gathercoal, K. A. (1999). Face perception during early infancy. Psychological Science, 10, 419–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1986). The infant’s role in mother-infant communication. Journal of Child Language, 13, 15–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., Tomonaga, M., Tanaka, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2004). Imitation in neonatal chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes). Developmental Science, 7, 437–442.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., & Tomonaga, M. (2001). Development of face recognition in an infant gibbon (Hylobates agilis). Infant Behavior and Development, 24, 215–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagell, K., Olguin, R. S., & Tomasello, M. (1993). Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 174–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, E. (2006). From imitation to conversation: The first dialogues with human neonates. Infant and Child Development, 15, 223–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, E., & Molnar, P. (2004). Homo imitans or homo provocans? Human imprinting model of neonatal imitation. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 54–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 555–565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M. (2012). Imitation, pretend play, and childhood: Essential elements in the evolution of human culture? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 170–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Overimitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 21, 729–736.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W. (1981). Ontogenetic adaptations and retrogressive processes in the development of the nervous system and behavior. In K. J. Connolly & H. F. R. Prechtl (Eds.), Maturation and development: Biological and psychological perspectives (pp. 73–108). Philadelphia: International Medical Publications

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation (Vol. 24). New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, J., Harrison, D., & Klima, E. S. (1995). Emotional talk and talk about emotions. Genetic Counseling, 6, 158–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosati, A., Hare, B. A., & Santos, L. R. (2010). Primate social cognition: Thirty years after Premack and Woodruff. Primate Neuroethology, 1, 117–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind a comparison of Chinese and US preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17, 74–81.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simion, F., Regolin, L., & Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 809–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slater, A., Von der Schulenburg, C., Brown, E., Badenoch, M., Butterworth, G., Parsons, S., & Samuels, C. (1998). Newborn infants prefer attractive faces. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 345–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slater, A., Quinn, P. C., Hayes, R., & Brown, E. (2000). The role of facial orientation in newborn infants’ preference for attractive faces. Developmental Science, 3, 181–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2005). The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees: III. Understanding intentional action. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10, 121–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Savage Rumbaugh, S., & Kruger, A. C. (1993). Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child development, 64(6), 1688–1705.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993a). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., & Kruger, A. C. (1993b). Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Development, 64, 1688–1705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walton, G. E., Bower, N. J. A., & Bower, T. G. R. (1992). Recognition of familiar faces by newborns. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 265–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). The emergence of contingent reciprocity in young children. Journal of Experimental child Psychology, 116, 338–350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., & Flynn, E. (2010). The transmission and evolution of experimental microcultures in groups of young children. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1694–1709.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williamson, R. A., & Markman, E. M. (2006). Precision of imitation as a function of preschoolers’ understanding of the goal of the demonstration. Developmental Psychology, 42, 723–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, S., & Keysar, B. (2007). The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychological Science, 18, 600–606.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, J., & Johnson, S. C. (2009). Biological motion displays elicit social behavior in 12-month-olds. Child Development, 80, 1069–1075.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karin Machluf Ph.D. .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Machluf, K., Bjorklund, D. (2015). Social Cognitive Development from an Evolutionary Perspective. In: Zeigler-Hill, V., Welling, L., Shackelford, T. (eds) Evolutionary Perspectives on Social Psychology. Evolutionary Psychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-12697-5_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics