Skip to main content

Affect, Availability and Presence

  • Chapter
Immersed in Media

Abstract

This chapter is intended to be both theoretical and a little speculative. It draws upon psychological, neuro-dynamic and philosophical sources to create an account of what happens when we experience presence, that is, when we become aware that we are present. This chapter also offers an alternate treatment of the work of Riva and his colleagues with respect to their bio-cultural approach to presence.

Rather than appealing to evolution, biology or technology, this account starts like this: affect precedes cognition. We feel before we think. These feelings (or affective responses) are primarily evaluative and effectively prime our cognition for the world (real or digital) we find ourselves in. All of the apparatus of sense making, reasoning and so forth follow fairly quickly but they are not the first responders. Our affective response is very fast – much faster than our cognition.

In answering the question, how do we find the world, that is, just what is this affective response in response to, we must switch from psychology to philosophy.

Heidegger tells us that we encounter the world as available. Psychology and sense making follow this. We connect with this available world by what Merleau-Ponty calls an intentional arc or intentional threads which “anchor us” to it. This is not simply philosophical discourse as Freeman is able to explain the neuro-dynamics of this arc by invoking the operation of the limbic system – that is, those parts of the brain responsible for our emotional response to the world.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    It should be noted that in their more recent treatments of presence, Riva and Waterworth have modified and significantly extended their position. For example, they treat extended presence as, “related to the emergence of the extended self: the intuitive perception of successfully acting in the external world towards a possible object” (Riva and Waterworth 2013, p. 207).

    From this short quotation we can see that they propose that presence is intuitive and is concerned with goal-directed behaviour. By intuitive, they mean that the experience of presence, as a product of our cognition, does not require conscious deliberation while the term object carrying with it the suggestion of objective, motivation and, possibly, intentionality. Here presence is seen to have an evaluative role, providing the organism with feedback on how it is doing with regard to achieving its object or goals. Riva and Waterworth have also suggested a neurological mechanism for this involving a particular form of simulation theory (cf. Blackemore and Decety 2001).

  2. 2.

    To illustrate the importance of recognizing presence as intentional it is worth contrasting it with a non-intentional state. One such non-intentional state is anxiety. Anxiety is, by definition, a sense or state of nervousness or unease about something uncertain or ill-defined, for example, “I am anxious about the state of the world”. Presence is about feeling one is somewhere or engaged with something in particular.

References

  • Ballew, C. C., II, & Todorov, A. (2007). Predicting political elections from rapid and unreflective face judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(46), 17948–17953.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basso, A., Goldberg, D., Greenspan, S., & Weimer, D. (2001). First impressions: Emotional and cognitive factors underlying judgments of trust in e-commerce. In Proceedings of the 3rd ACM conference on electronic commerce (pp. 137–143). New York: ACM Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, D. S. (2000). Attractiveness, attraction, and sexual selection: Evolutionary perspectives on the form and function of physical attractiveness. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 273–342). San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackemore, S. J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 561–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borkenau, P., Brecke, S., Möttig, C., & Paelecke, M. (2009). Extraversion is accurately perceived after a 50-ms exposure to a face. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 703–706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, R. F. (1992). Subliminal mere exposure effects. In R. F. Bornstein & T. S. Pittman (Eds.), Perception without awareness. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, S., Robillard, G., Marchand, A., Renaud, P., & Riva, G. (2005). Presence and the bond between patients and their psychotherapists in the cognitive-behavior therapy of panic disorder with agoraphobia delivered in videoconference. In 2007 10th Annual International Workshop on Presence, October 25–27, 2007 Barcelona, Spain (pp. 265–276).

    Google Scholar 

  • Brentano, F. C. (1874). Psychology from an empirical standpoint (A. C. Rancurello, D. B. Terrell & L. McAlister, Trans.). London: Routledge, 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brentano, F. (1995). Psychology from an empirical standpoint (A. C. Rancurello, D. B. Terrell & L. L. McAlister, Trans.). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlin, A. S., Hoffman, H. G., & Weghorst, S. (1997). Virtual reality and tactile augmentation in the treatment of spider phobia: A case study. Behavior Research and Therapy, 35, 153–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 40, pp. 61–149). New York: Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, M. R. (1986). Measuring the physical in physical attractiveness: Quasi-experiments on the sociobiology of female facial beauty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 925–935.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (2010). Self comes to mind: Constructing the conscious brain. London: William Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. L. (1991). Being-in-the-world: A commentary on Heidegger’s being and time, Division 1. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. L., & Wrathall, M. A. (2005). Introduction. In H. L. Dreyfus & M. A. Wrathall (Eds.), A companion to Heidegger. Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, W. J. (1995). Societies of brains. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, W. J. (1999). Consciousness, intentionality and causality. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(11–12), 143–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Palacios, A., Hoffman, H. G., Kwong See, S., Tsai, A., & Botella, C. (1998). Redefining therapeutic success with virtual reality exposure therapy. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 4(3), 341–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., Rydell, R. J., Vervliet, B., & De Houwer, J. (2010). Generalization versus contextualization in automatic evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 683.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldie, P. (2002). The emotions: A philosophical exploration. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2002). Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia, 40, 212–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harman, G. (2002). Tool being: Heidegger and the metaphysics of objects. Chicago: Open Court.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1927/1962). Being and time (J. Macquarrie & E. Robinson, Trans.). New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindgaard, G., Fernandes, G., Dudek, C., & Brown, J. (2006). Attention web designers: You have 50 milliseconds to make a good first impression! Behaviour & Information Technology, 25(2), 115–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945/1962). Phenomenology of perception (C. Smith, Trans.). London: Routledge Classics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1988). Psychology of everyday things. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (2004). Emotional design. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K. (1992). Best laid schemes: The psychology of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive account of emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 1, 29–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1963). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1973). The child’s conception of the world. London: Palladin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space. London: Routledge Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riva, G., & Waterworth, J. A. (2013). Being present in a virtual world. In M. Grimshaw (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of virtuality (pp. 205–221). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riva, G., Waterworth, J. A., & Waterworth, E. L. (2004). The layers of presence: A bio-cultural approach to understanding presence in natural and mediated environments. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 7(4), 405–419.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbaum, B. O., Hodges, L., Watson, B. A., Kessler, G. D., & Opdyke, D. (1996). Virtual reality exposure therapy in the treatment of fear of flying: A case report. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 477–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2008). Brief exposures: Male sexual orientation is accurately perceived at 50 ms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1100–1105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69(5), 378–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. T. (1992). Affordances and prospective control: An outline of the ontology. Ecological Psychology, 4(3), 173–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, W. H. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 683–703.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions making up your mind after a 100-Ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17(7), 592–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. M. (1897). Outlines of psychology (C. Hubbard Judd, Trans.). Available from http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Wundt/Outlines/sec15.htm. Last retrieved December 29, 2013.

  • Zahorik, P., & Jenison, R. L. (1998). Presence as being-in-the-world. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 7, 78–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Phil Turner .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Turner, P. (2015). Affect, Availability and Presence. In: Lombard, M., Biocca, F., Freeman, J., IJsselsteijn, W., Schaevitz, R. (eds) Immersed in Media. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-10190-3_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-10190-3_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-10189-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-10190-3

  • eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics