Advertisement

Cognitive Archaeology and the Cognitive Sciences

  • Frederick L. Coolidge
  • Thomas Wynn
  • Karenleigh A. Overmann
  • James M. Hicks
Chapter
Part of the Springer Series in Bio-/Neuroinformatics book series (SSBN, volume 3)

Abstract

Cognitive archaeology uses cognitive and psychological models to interpret the archaeological record. This chapter outlines several components that may be essential in building effective cognitive archaeological arguments. It also presents a two-stage perspective for the development of modern cognition, primarily based upon the work of Coolidge and Wynn. The first describes the transition from arboreal to terrestrial life in later Homo and the possible cognitive repercussions of terrestrial sleep. The second stage proposes that a genetic event may have enhanced working memory in Homo sapiens (specifically in terms of Baddeley’s multicomponent working memory model). The present chapter also reviews the archaeological and neurological bases for modern thinking, and the latter arguments are primarily grounded in the significance of the morphometric rescaling of the parietal lobes, which appears to have distinguished Homo sapiens from Neandertals.

Keywords

Cognitive archaeology Working memory Enhanced working memory Parietal lobes Intraparietal sulcus IPS Precuneus Homo sapiens Neandertals 

References

  1. Aboitiz F, Aboitiz S, Garcia R (2010) The phonological loop: a key innovation in human evolution. Curr Anthropol 51:S55–S65Google Scholar
  2. Adams AM, Gathercole S (2000) Limitations in working memory: implications for language development. Int J Lang Commun Disord 35:95–116Google Scholar
  3. Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL (2007) Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 45:1363–1377Google Scholar
  4. Allison T, Cicchetti DV (1976) Sleep in mammals: ecological and constitutional correlates. Science 194:732–734Google Scholar
  5. Ambrose S (2010) Coevolution of composite-tool technology, constructive memory, and language: implications for the evolution of modern human behavior. In: Wynn T, Coolidge F (eds) Working memory: beyond language and symbolism. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  6. Antón SC (2003) Natural history of Homo erectus. Yearb Phys Anthropol 46:126–170Google Scholar
  7. Antón SC, Swisher CC (2004) Early dispersals of Homo from Africa. Annu Rev Anthropol 33:271–296Google Scholar
  8. Antón S, Leonard W, Robertson M (2002) An ecomorphological model of the initial hominid dispersal from Africa. J Hum Evol 43:773–785Google Scholar
  9. Baddeley A, Hitch GJ (1974) Working memory. In: Bower GA (ed) Recent advances in learning and motivation. Academic Press, New York, pp 47–90Google Scholar
  10. Baddeley A, Logie R (1999) Working memory: the multiple-component model. In: Miyake A, Shah P (eds) Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 28–61Google Scholar
  11. Baddeley A (2000) The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci 4:417–423Google Scholar
  12. Baddeley A (2001) Is working memory working? Am Psychol 56:851–864Google Scholar
  13. Baddeley A (2007) Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  14. Berger TD, Trinkaus E (1995) Patterns of trauma among the Neandertals. J Archaeol Sci 22:841–852Google Scholar
  15. Berna F, Goldberg P, Horwitz LK, Brink J, Holt S, Bamford M, Chazan M (2012) Microstratigraphic evidence of in situ fire in the Acheulean strata of Wonderwerk Cave, Northern Cape province, South Africa. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:E1215–E1220Google Scholar
  16. Bleed P (1986) The optimal design of hunting weapons: maintainability or reliability? Am Antiq 51:737–747Google Scholar
  17. Botha R (2008) Prehistoric shell beads as a window on language evolution. Lang Commun 28:197–212Google Scholar
  18. Botha R (2012) Inferring modern language from ancient objects. In: Tallerman M, Gibson KR (eds) The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 312–393Google Scholar
  19. Bramble DM, Lieberman DE (2004) Endurance running and the evolution of Homo. Nature 432:345–352Google Scholar
  20. Braun DR, Plummer T, Ditchfield P, Ferraro JV, Maina D, Bishop LC et al (2008) Oldowan behavior and raw material transport: perspectives from the Kanjera Formation. J Archaeol Sci 35:2329–2345Google Scholar
  21. Bruner E (2003) Fossil traces of the human thought: paleoneurology and the evolution of the genus Homo. Rivista di Antropologia, J Anthropol Sci 81:29–56Google Scholar
  22. Bruner E (2004) Geometric morphometrics and paleoneurology: brain shape evolution in the genus Homo. J Hum Evol 47:279–303Google Scholar
  23. Bruner E (2010) Morphological differences in the parietal lobes within the human genus: a neurofunctional perspective. Curr Anthropol 51:S77–S88Google Scholar
  24. Bruner E, Holloway RL (2010) A bivariate approach to the widening of the frontal lobes in the genus Homo. J Hum Evol 58:138–146Google Scholar
  25. Bruner E, de la Cuétara JM, Holloway RL (2011) A bivariate approach to the variation of the parietal curvature in the genus Homo. Anat Rec 294:1548–1556Google Scholar
  26. Bruner E, Jacobs HI (2012) Alzheimer’s disease: the downside of a highly evolved parietal lobe? J Alzheimers Dis 35:227–240Google Scholar
  27. Bruner E, De Lázaro GR, De la Cuétara JM, Martín-Loeches M, Colom R et al. (2014) Midsagittal brain variation and MRI shape analysis of the precuneus in adult individuals. J Anat 224:1–10Google Scholar
  28. Bruner E, Manzi G, Arsuaga JL (2003) Encephalization and allometric trajectories in the genus Homo: evidence from the Neandertal and modern lineages. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:15335–15340Google Scholar
  29. Cachel S, Harris JWK (1998) The lifeways of Homo erectus inferred from archaeology and evolutionary ecology: a perspective from East Africa. In: Petraglia MD, Korisettar R (eds) Early human behavior in global context: the rise and diversity of the Lower Palaeolithic record. Routledge, London, pp 108–132Google Scholar
  30. Cairney SA, Durrant SJ, Hulleman J, Lewis PA (2014) Targeted memory reactivation during slow wave sleep facilitates emotional memory consolidation. Sleep 37:701–707 Google Scholar
  31. Caruana MV, d’Errico F, Backwell L (2013) Early hominin social learning strategies underlying the use and production of bone and stone tools. In: Sanz CM, Call J, Boesch C (eds) Tool use in animals: cognition and ecology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 242–285Google Scholar
  32. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR (2006) The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioral correlates. Brain 129:564–583Google Scholar
  33. Chiappe DL, Chiappe P (2007) The role of working memory in metaphor production and comprehension. J Mem Lang 56:172–188Google Scholar
  34. Churchill SE (2001) Hand morphology, manipulation, and tool use in Neandertals and early modern humans of the Near East. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:2953–2955Google Scholar
  35. Clayton NS, Bussey TJ, Dickinson A (2003) Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Nat Rev Neurosci 4:685–691Google Scholar
  36. Collette F, Van der Linden M (2002) Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26:102–125Google Scholar
  37. Conneller C (2011) An archaeology of materials: substantial transformations in early prehistoric Europe. Routledge, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  38. Coolidge FL, Overmann KA (2012) Numerosity, abstraction, and the emergence of symbolic thinking. Curr Anthropol 53:204–225Google Scholar
  39. Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2001) Executive functions of the frontal lobes and the evolutionary ascendancy of Homo sapiens. Camb Archaeol J 11:255–260Google Scholar
  40. Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2005) Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Camb Archaeol J 15:5–26Google Scholar
  41. Coolidge FL (2007) The working memory account of Neandertal cognition—How phonological storage capacity may be related to recursion and the pragmatics of modern speech. J Hum Evol 52:707–710Google Scholar
  42. Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2009) The rise of Homo sapiens: the evolution of modern thinking. Wiley, HobokenGoogle Scholar
  43. Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2012) Cognitive prerequisites for a language of diplomacy. In: Tallerman M, Gibson KR (eds) The oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 216–223Google Scholar
  44. Coolidge FL, Overmann KA, Wynn T (2011) Recursion: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 2:547–554Google Scholar
  45. Culham JC, Kanwisher NG (2001) Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex. Cogn Neurosci 11:157–163Google Scholar
  46. d’Errico F, Henshilwood C, Vanhaeren M, Van Niekerk K (2005) Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the middle stone age. J Hum Evol 48:3–24Google Scholar
  47. Davidson I, Noble W (1989) The archaeology of perception: traces of depiction and language. Curr Anthropol 30:125–155Google Scholar
  48. Davidson I, Noble W (1993) Tools and language in human evolution. In: Gibson K, Ingold T (eds) Tools, language, and cognition in human evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 263–288Google Scholar
  49. Dehaene S (2011) The number sense, 2nd edn. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  50. Dehaene S, Brannon EM (Eds) (2011) Space, time, and number: searching for the foundations of mathematical thought. Academic Press, New York Google Scholar
  51. De Smet HJ, Paquier P, Verhoeven J, Mariën P (2013) The cerebellum: its role in language and related cognitive and affective functions. Brain Lang 127:334–342Google Scholar
  52. Domínguez-Rodrigo M, Pickering TR, Bunn HT (2010) Configurational approach to identifying the earliest hominin butchers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:20929–20934Google Scholar
  53. Donald M (1991) Origins of the modern mind: three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  54. Dunbar RIM (2003) The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annu Rev Anthropol 32:163–181Google Scholar
  55. Fernández-Laso MC, Navarro MGC, García-Antón MD, Rivals F (2011) Territorial mobility of Neanderthal groups: a case study from level M of Abric Romaní (Capellades, Barcelona, Spain). In: Conard NJ, Richter J (eds) Neanderthal lifeways, subsistence and technology: 150 years of Neanderthal study. Springer, Dordrecht, pp 187–202Google Scholar
  56. Franklin MS, Zyphur MJ (2005) The role of dreams in the evolution of the human mind. Evol Psychol 3:59–78Google Scholar
  57. Fruth B, Hohmann G (1996) Nest building behavior in the great apes: the great leap forward? In: McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T (eds) Great ape societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 225–239Google Scholar
  58. Gais S, Plihal W, Wagner U, Born J (2000) Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills. Nat Neurosci 3:1335–1339Google Scholar
  59. García-Antón MD, Granda LM, Navarro MGC (2011) Level G of Las Fuentes de San Cristóbal (Southern Pyrenees, Spain): availability of lithic resources and territory management. In: Conard NJ, Richter J (eds) Neanderthal lifeways, subsistence and technology: 150 years of Neanderthal Study. Springer, Dordrecht, pp 203–219Google Scholar
  60. Gazzaniga M, Ivry R, Mangun G (2013) Cognitive neuroscience, 4th edn. W.W. Norton & Co, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  61. Geva S, Jones PS, Crinion JT, Price CJ, Baron J-C, Warburton EA (2011) The neural correlates of inner speech defined by voxel-based lesion symptom mapping. Brain 134:3071–3082Google Scholar
  62. Graves RR, Lupo AC, McCarthy RC, Wescott DJ, Cunningham DL (2010) Just how strapping was KNM-WT 15000? J Hum Evol 59:542–554Google Scholar
  63. Gunz P, Neubauer S, Maureille B, Hublin JJ (2010) Brain development after birth differs between Neanderthals and modern humans. Curr Biol 20:R921–R922Google Scholar
  64. Hartmann E (1998) Dreams and nightmares: the new theory on the origin and meaning of dreams. Plenum Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  65. Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WT (2002) The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298:1569–1579Google Scholar
  66. Hazy T, Frank M, O’Reilly R (2006) Banishing the homunculus: making working memory work. Neuroscience 139:105–118Google Scholar
  67. Henshilwood C, Dubreuil B (2009) Reading the artefacts: Gleaning language skills from the Middle Stone Age in southern Africa. In: Botha R (ed) The cradle of language, Stellenbosch, South Africa, pp 41–63Google Scholar
  68. Henshilwood CS, d’Errico F, Vanhaeren M, van Niekerk K, Jacobs Z (2004) Middle Stone Age shell beads from South Africa. Science 304:404Google Scholar
  69. Hodgson D (2009) Evolution of the visual cortex and the emergence of symmetry in the Acheulean techno-complex. CR Palevol 8:93–97Google Scholar
  70. Holloway RL (1996) Toward a synthetic theory of human brain evolution. In: Changeaux JP, Chavaillon J (eds) Origins of the human brain. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 42–54Google Scholar
  71. Kane M, Engle R (2002) The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychon Bull Rev 9:637–671Google Scholar
  72. Karni A, Tanne D, Rubenstein BS, Askenasy JJ, Sagi D (1994) Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. Science 265:679–682Google Scholar
  73. Keller C, Keller J (1996) Cognition and tool use: the blacksmith at work. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  74. Klein R (2000) Archeology and the evolution of human behavior. Evol Anthropol 9:17–36Google Scholar
  75. Koenigs M, Barbey AK, Postle BR, Grafman J (2009) Superior parietal cortex is critical for the manipulation of information in working memory. J Neurosci 29:14980–14986Google Scholar
  76. Kohn M, Mithen S (1999) Handaxes: products of sexual selection? Antiquity 73:518–526Google Scholar
  77. Koziol LF, Budding DE, Chidekel D (2010) Adaptation, expertise, and giftedness: towards an understanding of cortical, subcortical, and cerebellar network contributions. Cerebellum 9:499–529Google Scholar
  78. Krippner S, Hughes W (1970) Genius at work. Psychol Today June 40–43Google Scholar
  79. Land MF (2014) Do we have an internal model of the outside world? Philos Trans R Soc B 369:20130045Google Scholar
  80. Lezak M (1982) The problem of assessing executive functions. Int J Psychol 17:281–297Google Scholar
  81. Lieberman DE, Bramble DM, Raichlen DA, Shea JJ (2009) Brains, brawn and the evolution of human endurance running capabilities. In: Grine FE, Fleagle JG, Leakey RE (eds) The first humans: origin and early evolution of the genus Homo. Springer, New York pp 77–98 Google Scholar
  82. Lombard M, Haidle M (2012) Thinking a bow-and-arrow set: cognitive implications of Middle Stone Age bow and stone-tipped arrow technology. Camb Archaeol J 22:237–264Google Scholar
  83. Lou HC, Luber B, Crupain M, Keenan JP, Nowak M, Kjaer TW et al (2004) Parietal cortex and representation of the mental self. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:6827–6832Google Scholar
  84. Malafouris L (2008) Beads for a plastic mind: the ‘Blind Man’s Stick’ (BMS) hypothesis and the active nature of material culture. Camb Archaeol J 18:401–414Google Scholar
  85. Malafouris L (2010) Grasping the concept of number: how did the sapient mind move beyond approximation? In: Renfrew C, Morley I (eds) The archaeology of measurement: comprehending heaven, earth and time in ancient societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 35–42Google Scholar
  86. Malafouris L (2013) How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  87. Malcolm-Smith S, Solms M (2004) Incidence of threat in dreams: a response to Revonsuo’s threat simulation theory. Dreaming 14:220–229Google Scholar
  88. Marean C (2010) Introduction to the special issue: the Middle Stone Age at Pinnacle Point Site 13B, a coastal cave near Mossel Bay (Western Cape Province, South Africa). J Hum Evol 59:231–233Google Scholar
  89. Martin-Ordas G, Call J (2013) Episodic memory: a comparative approach. Front Behav Neurosci 7:1–13Google Scholar
  90. McGrew WC (1992) Chimpanzee material culture. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  91. McGrew WC (2004) The cultured chimpanzee: reflections on cultural primatology. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  92. McPherron SP (2000) Handaxes as a measure of the mental abilities of early hominids. J Archaeol Sci 27:655–663Google Scholar
  93. McPherron SP, Alemseged Z, Marean CW, Wynn JG, Reed D, Geraads D et al. (2010) Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature 466:857–860Google Scholar
  94. Mithen S (1999) Imitation and cultural change: a view from the Stone Age, with specific reference to the manufacture of handaxes. In: Box HO, Gibson KR (eds) Mammalian social learning: comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 389–399Google Scholar
  95. Miyake A, Shah P (1999) Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  96. Neubauer S, Gunz P, Hublin JJ (2009) The pattern of endocranial ontogenetic shape changes in humans. J Anat 215:240–255Google Scholar
  97. Neubauer S, Gunz P, Hublin JJ (2010) Endocranial shape changes during growth in chimpanzees and humans: a morphometric analysis of unique and shared aspects. J Hum Evol 59:555–566Google Scholar
  98. Noble W, Davidson I (1996) Human evolution, language, and mind: a psychological and archaeological inquiry. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  99. O’Connell JF, Allen J (2004) Dating the colonization of Sahul (Pleistocene Australia-New Guinea): a review of recent research. J Archaeol Sci 31:835–853Google Scholar
  100. Ogawa A, Yamazaki Y, Ueno K, Cheng K, Iriki A (2010) Inferential reasoning by exclusion recruits parietal and prefrontal cortices. Neuroimage 52:1603–1610Google Scholar
  101. Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, Vitiello MV (2004) Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep 27:1255–127Google Scholar
  102. Orban GA (2011) The extraction of 3D shape in the visual system of human and nonhuman primates. Annu Rev Neurosci 34:361–388Google Scholar
  103. Orban GA, Caruana F (2014) The neural basis of human tool use. Front Psychol 5:1–12Google Scholar
  104. Osaka N, Logie R, d’Esposito M (eds) (2007) The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  105. Oswalt W (1976) An anthropological analysis of food-getting technology. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  106. Overmann KA (2014) Finger-counting in the Upper Paleolithic. Rock Art Research 31:63–80Google Scholar
  107. Peeters RR, Simone L, Nelissen K, Fabbri-Destro M, Vanduffel W, Rizzolatti G et al (2009) The representation of tool use in humans and monkeys: common and uniquely human features. J Neurosci 29:11523–11539Google Scholar
  108. Peeters RR, Rizzolatti G, Orban GA (2013) Functional properties of the left parietal tool use region. Neuroimage 78:83–93Google Scholar
  109. Ramachandran VS (2004) A brief tour of human consciousness: from impostor poodles to purple numbers. Pi Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  110. Reuland E (2005) On the evolution and genesis of language: the force of imagination. Lingue e Linguaggio 4:81–110Google Scholar
  111. Revonsuo A, Valli K (2000) Dreaming and consciousness: testing the threat simulation theory of the function of dreaming. Psyche 6 [Internet]. 2000 [cited 2014 January 11]. http://www.theassc.org/files/assc/2467.pdf
  112. Revonsuo A (2000) The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behav Brain Sci 23:793–1121Google Scholar
  113. Roebroeks W, Kolen J, Rensink E (1988) Planning depth, anticipation and the organization of Middle Palaeolithic technology: “Archaic natives” meet Eve’s descendents. Helenium 28:17–34Google Scholar
  114. Rouillon A (2006) Au Gravettien, dans la grotte Cosquer (Marseille, Bouches-du-Rhône), l’Homme a-t-il compté sur ses doigts? L’anthropologie 110:500–509Google Scholar
  115. Ruff CB (1991) Climate and body shape in hominid evolution. J Hum Evol 21:81–104Google Scholar
  116. Ruff CB (1993) Climatic adaptation and hominid evolution: the thermoregulatory imperative. Evol Anthropol 2:53–60 Google Scholar
  117. Sabater Pi J, Veá JJ, Serrallonga J (1997) Did the first hominids build nests? Curr Anthropol 38:914–916Google Scholar
  118. Savage-Rumbaugh ES, Toth N, Schick K (2007) Kanzi learns to knap stone tools. In: Washburn DA (ed) Primate perspectives on behavior and cognition. American Psychological Association, Washington, pp 279–291Google Scholar
  119. Schick K, Toth N (2006) An overview of the Oldowan industrial complex: the sites and the nature of their evidence. In: Toth N, Schick K (eds) The Oldowan: case studies into the earliest stone age. Stone Age Institute Press, Gosport, pp 3–42Google Scholar
  120. Schick K, Toth N (2009) The cutting edge: new approaches to the archaeology of human origins. Stone Age Institute Press, GosportGoogle Scholar
  121. Schlanger N (1996) Understanding Levallois: lithic technology and cognitive archaeology. Camb Archaeol J 6:231–254Google Scholar
  122. Segal DL, Coolidge FL, Rosowsky E (2006) Personality disorders and older adults: diagnosis, assessment, and treatment. John Wiley & Sons, HobokenGoogle Scholar
  123. Seghier ML (2012) The angular gyrus: multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. Neuroscientist 19:1–19Google Scholar
  124. Semaw S, Renne P, Harris JWK, Feibel CS, Bernor RL, Fesseha N, Mowbray K (1997) 2.5-million-year-old stone tools from Gona Ethiopia. Nature 385:333–336Google Scholar
  125. Semaw S, Rogers MJ, Quade J, Renne PR, Butler RF, Dominguez-Rodrigo M, Simpson SW (2003) 2.6-million-year-old stone tools and associated bones from OGS-6 and OGS-7, Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. J Hum Evol 45:169–177Google Scholar
  126. Semaw S, Rogers M, Stout D (2009) Insights into late Pliocene lithic assemblage variability: the East Gona and Ounda Gona South Oldowan archaeology (2.6 million years ago), Afar, Ethiopia. In: Schick K, Toth N (eds) The cutting edge: new approaches to the archaeology of human origins. Stone Age Institute Press, Gosport, pp 211–246Google Scholar
  127. Sherratt A (1997) Climatic cycles and behavioural revolutions: the emergence of modern humans and the beginning of farming. Antiquity 71:187–271Google Scholar
  128. Spinapolice EE (2012) Raw material economy in Salento (Apulia, Italy): new perspectives on Neanderthal mobility patterns. J Archaeol Sci 39:680–689Google Scholar
  129. Spoor F, Zonneveld F (1998) Comparative review of the human bony labyrinth Yearb Phys Anthropol 41:211–251Google Scholar
  130. Stanley SM (1992) An ecological theory for the origin of Homo. Paleobiology 18:237–257Google Scholar
  131. Stickgold R, James L, Hobson JA (2000) Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training. Nat Neurosci 2:1237–1238Google Scholar
  132. Steudel-Numbers K (2006) Energetics in Homo erectus and other early hominins: the consequences of increased lower limb length. J Hum Evol 51:445–453Google Scholar
  133. Stout D (2002) Skill and cognition in stone tool production. Curr Anthropol 43:693–722Google Scholar
  134. Stout D, Chaminade T (2007) The evolutionary neuroscience of tool making. Neuropsychologia 45:1091–1100Google Scholar
  135. Stout D, Passingham R, Frith C, Apel J, Chaminade T (2011) Technology, expertise, and social cognition in human evolution. Eur J Neurosci 33:1328–1338Google Scholar
  136. Stout D, Toth N, Schick K, Chaminade T (2008) Neural correlates of Early Stone Age toolmaking: technology, language, and cognition in human cognition. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 363:1939–1949Google Scholar
  137. Stout D, Toth N, Schick K, Stout J, Hutchins G (2000) Stone tool-making and brain activation: positron emission tomography (PET) studies. J Archaeol Sci 27:1215–1223Google Scholar
  138. Suddendorf T, Corballis MC (2007) The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behav Brain Sci 30:299–351Google Scholar
  139. Tononi G, Cirelli C (2014) Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81:12–34Google Scholar
  140. Toth N (1985) The Oldowan reassesed: a closer look at early stone artifacts. J Archaeol Sci 12:101–120Google Scholar
  141. Toth N (1987) Behavioral inferences from Early Stone Age artifact assemblages: an experimental model. J Hum Evol 16:763–787Google Scholar
  142. Toth N, Schick K (2009) The Oldowan: the tool making of early hominins and chimpanzees compared. Annu Rev Anthropol 38:289–305Google Scholar
  143. Toth N, Schick K (eds) (2006) The Oldowan: case studies into the earliest stone age. Stone Age Institute Press, GosportGoogle Scholar
  144. Trinkaus E (1995) Neanderthal mortality patterns. J Archaeol Sci 22:121–142Google Scholar
  145. Trinkaus E (2012) Neandertals, early modern humans, and rodeo riders. J Archaeol Sci 39:3691–3693Google Scholar
  146. Tulving E (2002) Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol 53:1–25Google Scholar
  147. Twomey T (2013) The cognitive implications of controlled use of fire. Camb Archaeol J 23:113–128Google Scholar
  148. Valli K, Revonsuo A, Pälkäs O, Ismahil KH, Ali KJ, Punamäki R-L (2005) The threat simulation theory of the evolutionary function of dreaming: evidence from dreams of traumatized children. Conscious Cogn 14:188–218Google Scholar
  149. Van de Castle RL (1983) Animal figures in fantasy and dreams. In: Kacher A, Beck A (eds) New perspectives on our lives with companion animals. University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia, pp 148–173Google Scholar
  150. Vandervert LR (2003a) How working memory and cognitive modeling functions of the cerebellum contribute to discoveries in mathematics. New Ideas Psychol 21:159–175Google Scholar
  151. Vandervert LR (2003b) The neurophysiological basis of innovation. In: Shavinina LV (ed) The international handbook on innovation. Elsevier Science, Oxford, pp 17–30Google Scholar
  152. Vandervert LR (2009) The appearance of the child prodigy 10,000 years ago: an evolutionary and developmental explanation. J Mind Behav 30:15–32Google Scholar
  153. Vandervert LR (2011) The evolution of language: the cerebro-cerebellar blending of visual-spatial working memory with vocalizations. J Mind Behav 32:317–332Google Scholar
  154. Vandervert LR, Schimpf PH, Liu H (2007) How working memory and the cerebellum collaborate to produce creativity and innovation. Creat Res J 19:1–18Google Scholar
  155. Vanhaeren M, d’Errico F (2006) Aurignacian ethno-linguistic geography of Europe revealed by personal ornaments. J Archaeol Sci 33:1105–1128Google Scholar
  156. Vanhaeren M, d’Errico F, Van Niekerk K, Henshilwood C, Erasmus R (2013) Thinking strings: additional evidence for personal ornament use in the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa. J Hum Evol 64:500–517Google Scholar
  157. Wadley L (2010) Were snares and traps used in the Middle Stone Age and does it matter? a review and a case study from Sibudu, South Africa. J Hum Evol 58:179–192Google Scholar
  158. Wadley L, Hodgskiss T, Grant M (2009) Implications for complex cognition from the hafting of tools with compound adhesives in the Middle Stone Age, South Africa. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:9590–9594Google Scholar
  159. Wagner U, Gais S, Haider H, Verleger R, Born J (2004) Sleep inspires insight. Nature 427:352–355Google Scholar
  160. Weaver AH (2005) Reciprocal evolution of the cerebellum and neocortex in fossil humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:3576–3580Google Scholar
  161. Whiten A, Byrne R (1991) The emergence of metarepresentation in human ontogeny and primate phylogeny. In: Whiten A (ed) Natural theories of mind. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 267–281Google Scholar
  162. Whiten A, Ham R (1992) On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: reappraisal of a century of research. In: Slater P, Rosenblatt J, Beer J, Milinski M (ed) Advances in the study of behavior, Academic Press, New York, vol 21, pp 239–283Google Scholar
  163. Winson J (1990) The meaning of dreams. Sci Am 263:89–96Google Scholar
  164. Wong K (2006) Lucy’s baby. Sci Am 295:78–85Google Scholar
  165. Woods AD (2011) The effects of lithic raw material quality on Aurignacian blade production at Abri Cellier. Doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa. http://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/1111
  166. Wynn T (1981) The intelligence of Oldowan hominids. J Hum Evol 10:529–541Google Scholar
  167. Wynn T (1995) Handaxe enigmas. World Archaeol 27:10–24Google Scholar
  168. Wynn T (2002) Archaeology and cognitive evolution. Behav Brain Sci 25:389–438Google Scholar
  169. Wynn T (2009a) Hafted spears and the archaeology of mind. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:9544–9545Google Scholar
  170. Wynn T (2009b) Whither evolutionary cognitive archaeology? Afterword. In: de Beaune S, Coolidge FL, Wynn T (eds) Cognitive archaeology and human evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 145–149Google Scholar
  171. Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2003) The role of working memory in the evolution of managed foraging. Before Farming 2:1–16Google Scholar
  172. Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2010) Beyond symbolism and language: an introduction to supplement 1, working memory. Curr Anthropol 51(Supplement1):S5–S16Google Scholar
  173. Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2011) The implications of the working memory model for the evolution of modern cognition. Int J Evol Biol [Internet]. 2011 [cited 2014 January 11]; Article ID 741357, p 12. doi: 10.4061/2011/741357
  174. Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2012) How to think like a Neandertal. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  175. Wynn T, Coolidge F, Bright M (2009) Hohlenstein-Stadel and the evolution of human conceptual thought. Camb Archaeol J 19(1):73–83Google Scholar
  176. Wynn T, Hernandez-Aguilar RA, Marchant L, McGrew WC (2011) “An ape’s view of the Oldowan” revisited. Evol Anthropol 20:181–197Google Scholar
  177. Zilhão J, Angelucci DE, Badal-Garcia E, d’Errico F, Daniel F, Dayet L, Zapata J (2010) Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:1023–1028Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  • Frederick L. Coolidge
    • 1
  • Thomas Wynn
    • 2
  • Karenleigh A. Overmann
    • 1
  • James M. Hicks
    • 1
  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentUniversity of ColoradoColorado SpringsUSA
  2. 2.Anthropology DepartmentUniversity of ColoradoColorado SpringsUSA

Personalised recommendations