Abstract
“Neuroarchaeology” in the broad sense refers to any application of neuroscience theory and methods to archaeological questions. This includes the interpretation of archaeological materials in terms of the cognitive operations and neural substrates they are thought to imply as well as the experimental study of archaeologically-visible behaviors using neuroscience methods. The particular strengths and interests of archaeology have led neuroarchaeologists to focus on three broad themes in neuroscience theory: grounded cognition, executive function, and social cognition. Much of the published work in neuroarchaeology has consisted of attempts to apply neuroscience perspectives on these topics to interpretations of the archaeological record. Experimental neuroarchaeology, a straightforward methodological extension of conventional experimental archaeology, has been less common. This may change with the increasing availability of neuroscience methods for investigating complex, real-world behaviors. The use of neuroimaging methods to study experimental stone tool-making provides one example. Archaeology and neuroscience are united in the quest to understand human nature but remain deeply divided by disciplinary history, culture, methods, career paths, and institutional support. Whereas the utility of neuroscience methods to archaeology is clear, there has been less interest among neuroscientists in the potential contributions of archaeological strengths in the study of evolution and material culture. The future of “neuroarchaeology” as just another niche focus within archaeology or as something more will ultimately depend on its relevance to the questions and research agendas of neuroscientists.
Keywords
- Archaeology
- Cognition
- Neuroscience
- Stone tools
- Brain imaging
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
Buying options


References
Arbib MA (2011) From mirror neurons to complex imitation in the evolution of language and tool use. Ann Rev Anthropol 40(1):257–273
Aron AA, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA (2004) Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 8(4):170–177
Baddeley A (2003) Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci 4(10):829–839
Badre D, D’Esposito M (2009) Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nat Rev Neurosci 10(9):659–669
Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22(04):577–660
Barsalou LW (2008) Grounded cognition. Ann Rev Psychol 59:617–645
Bartels A, Zeki S (2004) Functional brain mapping during free viewing of natural scenes. Hum Brain Mapp 21(2):75–85
Battro AM, Fischer KW, and Léna PJ (2008) The educated brain: essays in neuroeducation: Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Bernstein NA (1996) On dexterity and its development. In: Latash ML (ed) Dexterity and its development. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, Mahwah, p 3–246
Berti A, Frassinetti F (2000) When far becomes near: remapping of space by tool use. J Cogn Neurosci 12(3):415–420
Bookheimer S (2002) Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Ann Rev Neurosci 25:151–188
Bril B, Roux V (2005a) General introduction: a dynamic systems framework for studying a uniquely hominin innovation. In: Roux V, Bril B (eds) Stone knapping: the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp 1–18
Bril B, Roux V (2005b) Synthesis and speculations. In: Roux V, Bril B (eds) Stone knapping: the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp 353–355
Bril B, Roux V, Dietrich G (2000) Habilites impliquees dans la taille des perles en roches dure: characteristiques motrices et cognitives d’une action situe complexe. In: Roux V (ed) Les perles de Cambay: des practiques techniques aux technosystemes de L’orient Ancien. Editions de la MSH, Paris, pp 211–329
Bril B, Roux V, Dietrich G (2005) Stone knapping: Khambhat (India), a unique opportunity? In: Roux V, Bril B (eds) Stone knapping: the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp 53–72
Bril B, Dietrich G, Foucart J, Fuwa K, Hirata S (2009) Tool use as a way to assess cognition: how do captive chimpanzees handle the weight of the hammer when cracking a nut? Anim Cogn 12(2):217–235
Bril B, Rein R, Nonaka T, Wenban-Smith F, Dietrich G (2010) The role of expertise in tool use: skill differences in functional action adaptations to task constraints. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36(4):825–839
Bril B, Smaers J, Steele J, Rein R, Nonaka T, Dietrich G, Biryukova E, Hirata S, Roux V (2012) Functional mastery of percussive technology in nut-cracking and stone-flaking actions: experimental comparison and implications for the evolution of the human brain. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 367(1585):59–74
Broca P (2006) Comments regarding the seat of the faculty of spoken language, followed by an observation of Aphemia (loss of speech). In: Grodzinsky Y, Amunts K (eds) Broca’s region. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 291–304
Bruner E (2010) Morphological differences in the Parietal Lobes within the human genus. Curr Anthropol 51(S1):S77–S88
Camerer C, Loewenstein G, Prelec D (2005) Neuroeconomics: how neuroscience can inform economics. J Econ Lit 43(1):9–64
Chung K, Wallace J, Kim SY, Kalyanasundaram S, Andalman AS, Davidson TJ, Mirzabekov JJ, Zalocusky KA, Mattis J, Denisin AK et al (2013) Structural and molecular interrogation of intact biological systems. Nature 497(7449):332–337
Churchland PS (1989) Neurophilosophy: toward a unified science of the mind-brain: MIT Press, Cambridge
Clark A (2008) Supersizing the mind: embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Clark A, Chalmers D (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58(1):7–19
Coles JM (1979) Experimental archaeology. Academic Press, London
Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2001) Executive functions of the frontal lobes and the evolutionary ascendancy of Homo sapiens. Camb Archaeol J 11(2):255–260
Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2005) Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Camb Archaeol J 15(1):5–26
Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2009) The rise of Homo sapiens: the evolution of modern thinking. Wiley-Blackwell, Chichester
Corballis MC (2003) From mouth to hand: gesture, speech, and the evolution of right handedness. Behav Brain Sci 26:199–260
David N, Kramer C (2001) Ethnoarchaeology in action. Cambridge University Press, New York
Decety J, Grèzes J (2006) The power of simulation: imagining one’s own and other’s behavior. Brain Res 1079(1):4–14
d’Errico F, Stringer CB (2011) Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 366(1567):1060
Dias AM (2010) The foundations of neuroanthropology. Front Evol Neurosci 2:5 doi: 10.3389/neuro.18.005.2010
Domínguez Duque JF, Turner R, Lewis ED, Egan G (2010) Neuroanthropology: a humanistic science for the study of the culture–brain nexus. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 5(2–3):138–147
Dosenbach NU, Fair DA, Cohen AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2008) A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn Sci 12(3):99–105
Engle RW, Tuholski SW, Laughlin JE, Conway ARA (1999) Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. J Exp Psychol Gen 128(3):309–331
Ericsson KA, Krampe RT, Tesch-Romer C (1993) The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol Rev 100(3):363–406
Fadiga L, Craighero L, D’Ausilio A (2009) Broca’s area in language, action, and music. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1169(1):448–458
Fagg AH, Arbib MA (1998) Modeling parietal-premotor interactions in primate control of grasping. Neural Networks 11(7–8):1277–1303
Faisal A, Stout D, Apel J, Bradley B (2010) The manipulative complexity of lower paleolithic stone toolmaking. PLoS ONE 5(11):e13718
Ferrari M, Quaresima V (2012) A brief review on the history of human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. Neuroimage 63(2):921–935
Fink GR, Manjaly ZM, Stephan KE, Gurd JM, Zilles K, Amunts K, Marshall JC (2006) A role for Broca’s area beyond language processing: evidence from neuropsychology and fMRI. In: Godzinsky Y, Amunts K (eds) Broca’s region. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 254–268
Fragaszy DM (2011) Community resources for learning: how capuchin monkeys construct technical traditions. Biol Theory 6(3):231–240
Frey SH (2007) What puts the how in where? tool use and the divided visual streams hypothesis. Cortex 43(3):368–375
Frey SH, Gerry V (2006) Modulation of neural activity during observational learning of action and their sequential orders. J Neurosci 26(51):13194–13201
Frith CD, Frith U (2006) The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50(4):531–534
Gallese V, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G (2004) A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 8(9):396–403
Gallese V, Rochat M, Cossu G, Sinigaglia C (2009) Motor cognition and its role in the phylogeny and ontogeny of action understanding. Dev Psychol 45(1):103–113
Gell A (1998) Art and agency: an anthropological theory. Clarendon Press, Oxford
Gibson JJ (1986) The ecological approach to visual perception. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale
Gopnik A, Wellman HM (1992) Why the child’s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind Lang 7(1–2):145–171
Gowlett JAJ (1986) Culture and conceptualisation: the Oldowan-Acheulian gradient. In: Bailey GN, Callow P (eds) Stone age prehistory: studies in memory of Charles Mcburney. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 243–260
Grafton ST (2009) Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to understand others. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1156:97–117 (The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience 2009)
Grèzes J, Decety J (2002) Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia 40(2):212–222
Hagoort P (2005) On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework. Trends Cogn Sci 9(9):416–423
Hartmann K, Goldenberg G, Daumuller M, Hermsdorfer J (2005) It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: the neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices. Neuropsychologia 43:625–637
Hasson U, Nir Y, Levy I, Fuhrmann G, Malach R (2004) Intersubject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision. Science 303(5664):1634–1640
Head H, Holmes G (1911) Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain 34(2–3):102–254
Hecht EE, Gutman DA, Preuss TM, Sanchez MM, Parr LA, Rilling JK (2012) Process versus product in social learning: comparative diffusion tensor imaging of neural systems for action execution-observation matching in macaques, chimpanzees, and humans. Cereb Cortex 23(5):1014–1024
Hecht EE, Murphy LE, Gutman DA, Votaw JR, Schuster DM, Preuss TM, Orban GA, Stout D, Parr LA (2013) Differences in neural activation for object-directed grasping in chimpanzees and humans. J Neurosci 33(35):14117–14134
Henshilwood CS, Dubreuil B (2011) The still bay and howiesons poort, 77–59 ka: symbolic material culture and the evolution of the mind during the African middle Stone Age. Curr Anthropol 52(3):361–400
Hewes GW (1973) Primate communication and the gestural origins of language. Curr Anthropol 14:5–24
Hihara S, Notoya T, Tanaka M, Ichinose S, Ojima H, Obayashi S, Fujii N, Iriki A (2006) Extension of corticocortical afferents into the anterior bank of the intraparietal sulcus by tool-use training in adult monkeys. Neuropsychologia 44:2636–2646
Hodder I (2012) Entangled: an archaeology of the relationships between humans and things. Wiley-Blackwell, Malden
Holloway R (1969) Culture: a human domain. Curr Anthropol 10:395–412
Hopkins WD, Taglialatela JP, Russell JL, Nir TM, Schaeffer J (2010) Cortical representation of lateralized grasping in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): a combined MRI and PET study. PLoS ONE 5(10):e13383
Hutchins E (1995) Cognition in the wild. MIT Press, Cambridge
Ingold T (2001) Beyond art and technology: the anthropology of skill. In Michael S (ed) Anthropological Perspectives on Technology, University of New Mexico Press, Albuquerque, pp. 17–31
Iriki A, Sakura O (2008) The neuroscience of primate intellectual evolution: natural selection and passive and intentional niche construction. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 363(1500):2229–2241
Iriki A, Tanaka M, Iwamura Y (1996) Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport 7(14):2325
Johnson-Frey SH, Newman-Norlund R, Grafton ST (2005) A distributed left hemisphere network active during planning of everyday tool use skills. Cereb Cortex 15(6):681–695
Jones EG, Mendell LM (1999) Assessing the decade of the Brain. Science 284(5415):739
Karmiloff-Smith A (1992) Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. MIT Press, Cambridge
Kelly AM, Garavan H (2005) Human functional neuroimaging of brain changes associated with practice. Cereb Cortex 15(8):1089–1102
Kilner JM, Neal A, Weiskopf N, Friston KJ, Frith CD (2009) Evidence of mirror neurons in human inferior frontal gyrus. J Neurosci 29(32):10153–10159
Kirsch W, Herbort O, Butz MV, Kunde W (2012) Influence of motor planning on distance perception within the peripersonal space. PLoS ONE 7(4):e34880
Koechlin E, Jubault T (2006) Broca’s area and the hierarchical organization of human behavior. Neuron 50(6):963–974
Lakoff G, Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Lave J, Wenger E (1991) Situated learning: legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Malafouris L (2004) The cognitive basis of material engagement: where brain, body and culture conflate. In: DeMarrais E, Gosden C, Renfrew C (eds) Rethinking materiality: the engagement of mind with the material world. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp 53–61
Malafouris L (2008) Between brains, bodies and things: tectonite awareness and the extended self. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 363(1499):1993–2002
Malafouris L (2009) “Neuroarchaeology”: exploring the links between neural and cultural plasticity. In: Joan YC (ed) Progress in Brain research: Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 253–261
Malafouris L (2010) The brain–artefact interface (BAI): a challenge for archaeology and cultural neuroscience. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 5(2–3):264–273
Malafouris L, Renfrew C (2008) Introduction. Camb Archaeol J 18(03):381–385
Maravita A, Iriki A (2004) Tools for the body (schema). Trends Cogn Sci 8(2):79–86
McClelland JL, Botvinick MM, Noelle DC, Plaut DC, Rogers TT, Seidenberg MS, Smith LB (2010) Letting structure emerge: connectionist and dynamical systems approaches to cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 14(8):348–356
Menenti L, Petersson KM, Scheeringa R, Hagoort P (2009) When elephants fly: differential sensitivity of right and left inferior frontal gyri to discourse and world knowledge. J Cogn Neurosci 21(12):2358–2368
Merleau-Ponty M (1962) Phenomenology of perception. Humanities Press, New York
Miller GA (2003) The cognitive revolution: a historical perspective. Trends Cogn Sci 7(3):141–144
Mithen S (1996) The Prehistory of the mind: the cognitive origins of art, religion and science. Thames and Hudson Ltd, London
Nonaka T, Bril B, Rein R (2010) How do stone knappers predict and control the outcome of flaking? Implications for understanding early stone tool technology. J Hum Evol 59(2):155–167
Oakley KP (1954) Skill as a human possession. In: Singer C, Holmyard EJ, Hall AR (eds) A history of technology volume I, from early times to fall of ancient empires. Clarendon press, Oxford, pp 1–37
Obayashi S, Suhara T, Kawabe K, Okauchi T, Maeda J, Akine Y, Onoe H, Iriki A (2001) Functional brain mapping of monkey tool use. Neuroimage 14(4):853–861
Odling-Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman MW (2003) Niche construction: the neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton
Onians J (2007) Neuroarthistory: from Aristotle and Pliny to Baxandall and Zeki. New Haven [Conn.]; Yale University Press, London
Orban GA, Claeys K, Nelissen K, Smans R, Sunaert S, Todd JT, Wardak C, Durand J-B, Vanduffel W (2006) Mapping the parietal cortex of human and non-human primates. Neuropsychologia 44:2647–2667
Parasuraman R, Rizzo M (2006) Neuroergonomics: the brain at work. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Parr LA, Hecht E, Barks SK, Preuss TM, Votaw JR (2009) Face processing in the chimpanzee Brain. Curr Biol 19(1):50–53
Peeters R, Simone L, Nelissen K, Fabbri-Destro M, Vanduffel W, Rizzolatti G, Orban GA (2009) The representation of tool use in humans and monkeys: common and uniquely human features. J Neurosci 29(37):11523–11539
Peeters RR, Rizzolatti G, Orban GA (2013) Functional properties of the left parietal tool use region. NeuroImage 78:83–93
Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG (2009) Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324(5932):1298–1301
Preuss TM, Qi H, Kaas JH (1999) Distinctive compartmental organization of human primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci 96(20):11601–11606
Pulvermüller F, Fadiga L (2010) Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nat Rev Neurosci 11(5):351–360
Ramachandran VS (2000) Mirror neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind “the great leap forward” in human evolution. Edge Foundation web site
Reed ES (1996) Encountering the world: toward an ecological psychology. Oxford University Press, New York
Rein R, Bril B, Nonaka T (2013) Coordination strategies used in stone knapping. Am J Phys Anthropol 150(4):539–550
Renfrew C (1994) Towards a cognitive archaeology. In: Renfrew C, Zubrow EBW (eds) The ancient mind: elements of cognitive archaeology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 3–12
Renfrew C (2004) Towards a theory of material engagement. In: DeMarrais E, Gosden C, Renfrew C (eds) Rethinking materiality: the engagement of mind with the material world. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp 23–31
Renfrew C (2008) Neuroscience, evolution and the sapient paradox: the factuality of value and of the sacred. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 363(1499):2041–2047
Rilling JK (2008) Neuroscientific approaches and applications within anthropology. Am J Phys Anthropol 137(S47):2–32
Rilling JK, Stout D (In press) Evolution of the neural bases of higher cognitive function in humans. In The cognitive neurosciences, 5th edn
Rilling JK, Barks SK, Parr LA, Preuss TM, Faber TL, Pagnoni G, Bremner JD, Votaw JR (2007) A comparison of resting-state brain activity in humans and chimpanzees. Proc Natl Acad Sci 104(43):17146–17151
Rizzolatti G, Arbib MA (1998) Language within our grasp. Trends Cogn Sci 21(5):188–194
Rizzolatti G, Craighero L (2004) The mirror-neuron system. Ann Rev Neurosci 27:169–192
Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V (1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of actions. Cogn Brain Res 3:131–141
Rizzolatti G, Luppino G, Matelli M (1998) The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 106(4):283–296
Roepstorff A, Frith C (2012) Neuroanthropology or simply anthropology? Going experimental as method, as object of study, and as research aesthetic. Anthropol Theory 12(1):101–111
Rogoff B (1990) Apprenticeship in thinking: cognitive development in social context. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Ross ED, Monnot M (2008) Neurology of affective prosody and its functional-anatomic organization in right hemisphere. Brain Lang 104(1):51–74
Rossano MJ (2003) Expertise and the evolution of consciousness. Cognition 89(3):207–236
Roux V (1990) The psychological analysis of technical activities: a contribution to the study of craft specialization. Archaeol Rev Camb 9(1):142–153
Roux V, Bril B (eds) (2005) Stone knapping: the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge
Roux V, Bril B, Dietrich G (1995) Skills and learning difficulties involved in stone knapping. World Archaeol 27(1):63–87
Saraydar SC (2008) Replicating the past: the art and science of the archaeological experiment. Waveland Press, Long Grove, Ill
Saxe R (2005) Against simulation: the argument from error. Trends Cogn Sci 9(4):174–179
Schiffer MB (1999) The material life of human beings: artifacts, behavior and communication. Routledge, New York
Schlegel AA, Rudelson JJ (2012) White matter structure changes as adults learn a second language. J Cogn Neurosci 24(8):1664–1670
Scholz J, Klein MC, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H (2009) Training induces changes in white-matter architecture. Nat Neurosci 12(11):1370–1371
Schubotz RI, Cramon von DY (2003) Functional-anatomical concepts of human premotor cortex: evidence from fMRI and PET studies. NeuroImage 20: S120–S131
Semaw S (2000) The world’s oldest stone artefacts from Gona, Ethiopia: their implications for understanding stone technology and patterns of human evolution 2.6–1.5 million years ago. J Archaeol Sci 27:1197–1214
Stout D (2002) Skill and cognition in stone tool production: an ethnographic case study from Irian Jaya. Curr Anthropol 45(3):693–722
Stout D (2005) The social and cultural context of stone-knapping skill acquisition. In: Roux V, Bril B (eds) Stone knapping: the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp 331–340
Stout D (2010) The evolution of cognitive control. Topics Cogn Sci 2(4):614–630
Stout D (2011) Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 366(1567):1050–1059
Stout D, Chaminade T (2007) The evolutionary neuroscience of tool making. Neuropsychologia 45:1091–1100
Stout D, Chaminade T (2012) Stone tools, language and the brain in human evolution. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 367(1585):75–87
Stout D, Toth N, Schick KD, Chaminade T (2008) Neural correlates of Early Stone Age tool-making: technology, language and cognition in human evolution. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 363:1939–1949
Stout D, Passingham R, Frith C, Apel J, Chaminade T (2011) Technology, expertise and social cognition in human evolution. Eur J Neurosci 33(7):1328–1338
Stuss DT, Alexander MP (2007) Is there a dysexecutive syndrome? Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 362(1481):901–915
Taglialatela JP, Russell JL, Schaeffer JA, Hopkins WD (2008) Communicative signaling activates ‘Broca’s’ homolog in chimpanzees. Curr Biol 18(5):343–348
Taglialatela JP, Russell JL, Schaeffer JA, Hopkins WD (2009) Visualizing vocal perception in the chimpanzee brain. Cereb Cortex 19(5):1151–1157
Taglialatela JP, Russell JL, Schaeffer JA, Hopkins WD (2011) Chimpanzee vocal signaling points to a multimodal origin of human language. PLoS ONE 6(4):e18852
Teffer K, Semendeferi K (2012) Human prefrontal cortex: evolution, development, and pathology. In: Michel AH, Dean F (eds) Progress in Brain research. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 191–218
Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 364(1528):2405–2415
Thelen E, Smith L (1994) A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. MIT Press/Bradford Books, Cambridge
Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav Brain Sci 28(05):675–691
van Gelder T (1998) The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science. Behav Brain Sci 21:615–665
Vanduffel W, Fize D, Peuskens H, Denys K, Sunaert S, Todd JT, Orban GA (2002) Extracting 3D from motion: differences in human and monkey intraparietal cortex. Science 298:413–415
Vygotsky LS (1978) Mind in society: the development of higher psychological process. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Wadley L (2010) Compound-adhesive manufacture as a behavioral proxy for complex cognition in the Middle Stone Age. Curr Anthropol 51(S1):S111–S119
Wilson M (2002) Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon Bull Rev 9(4):625–636
Wolpert D, Doya K, Kawato M (2003) A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 358:593–602
Wynn T (1979) The intelligence of later acheulean hominids. Man 14(3):371–391
Wynn T (2002) Archaeology and cognitive evolution. Behav Brain Sci 25:389–438
Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2004) The expert Neandertal mind. J Hum Evol 46(4):467–487
Wynn T, Coolidge Frederick L (2010) Beyond symbolism and language: an introduction to supplement 1 working memory. Curr Anthropol 51(S1):S5–S16
Wynn T, Coolidge F, Bright M (2009) Hohlenstein-stadel and the evolution of human conceptual thought. Camb Archaeol J 19(01):73–84
Zukow-Goldring P, Arbib MA (2007) Affordances, effectivities, and assisted imitation: caregivers and the directing of attention. Neurocomputing 70(13–15):2181–2193
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland
About this chapter
Cite this chapter
Stout, D., Hecht, E. (2015). Neuroarchaeology. In: Bruner, E. (eds) Human Paleoneurology. Springer Series in Bio-/Neuroinformatics, vol 3. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-08500-5_7
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-08500-5_7
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-319-08499-2
Online ISBN: 978-3-319-08500-5
eBook Packages: EngineeringEngineering (R0)