Skip to main content

Abstract

Fairness or justice is central to moral development. A universal and impartial point of view on fairness has been considered as the final stage of life-span trajectories on moral development. Human rights are viewed as the core of such a fully developed morality. Although the study of fairness implies paying attention to trust, the relationship between fairness and trust has seldom been studied empirically. Fairness and trust are both necessary for and stimulated by cooperation. Fairness depends upon the development of social perspective-taking skills; empathy refers to the affective side of these skills. According to Gibbs’ developmental theory of moral motivation, the cognitive judgment of fairness and the affective response of empathy may both become a primary moral motive. Breaches of fairness or empathy can threaten or undermine interpersonal or social trust. In turn, disturbances in the development of trust in early childhood may have long-term negative consequences for the development of social perspective-taking skills and thus affect moral motivation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    During and after World War II, the intellectual and scientific community stood up to investigate how it could be possible for human beings to show that kind of “inhumanity” to their own mankind. Maslow for example wrote: “it was at that moment that I realized that the rest of my life must be devoted to discovering a psychology for the peace table” (Hoffman 1999, p. 137). After the war the international community vowed never again to allow atrocities like those of World War II happen again. As a result The United Nations was created, which developed The Universal Declaration of Human Rights (United Nations 2014a). The basic ideas are that human rights are rights inherent to all human beings, independent of our nationality, place of residence, sex, national or ethnic origin, color, religion, language, or any other status. All human beings are all equally entitled to our human rights without discrimination. These rights are all interrelated, interdependent and indivisible (United Nations 2014b). In the preamble of The Universal Declaration of Human Rights is stated that the “recognition of the inherent dignity and of the equal and inalienable rights of all members of the human family is the foundation of freedom, justice and peace in the world” (United Nations 2014a). Clearly, equality or fairness appears as a major principle. This is in accordance with for example article 1: “All human beings are born free and equal in dignity and rights” and article 7: “All are equal before the law and are entitled without any discrimination to equal protection of the law”. These human rights are central to (inter)national law, and can be viewed as a morality in which the consequences of impartiality and uniformity are fully represented.

  2. 2.

    For a critique of domain theory, see Gibbs 2010.

References

  • Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arsenio, W., & Lemerise, E. (2004). Aggression and moral development: Integrating the social information processing and moral domain models. Child Development, 75, 987–1002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Astor, R. A. (1994). Children’s moral reasoning about family and peer violence: The role of provocation and retaliation. Child Development, 65, 1054–1067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (2002). Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Moral Education, 31, 101–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1962). Social learning through Imitation. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barriga, A. Q., Morrison, E. M., Liau, A. K., & Gibbs, J. C. (2001). Moral cognition: Explaining the gender difference in antisocial behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47, 532–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basinger, K. S., & Gibbs, J. C. (1987). Validation of the sociomoral reflection objective measure-short form. Psychological Reports, 61, 139–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beerthuizen, M. G. C. J., & Brugman, D. (2012). Sexually abusive youths’ moral reasoning on sex. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 18, 125–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beerthuizen, M. G. C. J., & Brugman, D. (2013). Moral value evaluation: A neglected motivational concept in externalizing behaviour research. In K. Heinrichs, F. Oser, & T. Lovat (Eds.), Handbook of moral motivation: What makes people act morally right? Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernath, M. S., & Feshbach, N. D. (1995). Children’s trust: Theory, assessment, development, and research directions. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 4, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernath, M. S., Feshbach, N. D., & Gralinski, J. H. (1993). Physical maltreatment and trust in peers: Feelings, reasons, and behavioral intentions. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, March 25–28, 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  • Betts, L. R., & Rotenberg, K. J. (2008). A social relations analysis of children’s trust in their peers across the early years of school. Social Development, 17, 1039–1055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Betts, L. R., Rotenberg, K. J., & Trueman, M. (2009). The early childhood generalized trust belief scale. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 175–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R., Jones, L., Clark, F., & Smith, M. (1995). Is the psychopath ‘morally insane’? Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 741–752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R., Monson, J., & Fredrickson, N. (2001). Moral reasoning and conduct problems in children with emotional and behavioral difficulties. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 799–811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blasi, A. (1999). Emotions and moral motivation. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 29, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boom, J., Brugman, D., & van der Heijden, P. G. M. (2001). Hierarchical stage structure assessed by sorting moral statements. Child Development, 72, 535–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss, vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss, vol. 2: Separation, anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (1999). Internal working models in attachment relationships: a construct revisited. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (pp. 89–111). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brugman, D., Basinger, K. S., & Gibbs, J. C. (2007). Measuring adolescents’ moral judgment: an evaluation of the Sociomoral Reflection Measure—Short Form Objective (SRM-SFO). Paper presented at the Symposium: Cross-Cultural Research on Moral Reasoning. International Council of Psychologists, San Diego, USA, August 11–14, 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brugman, D., & Aleva, A. E. (2004). Developmental delay or regression in moral reasoning by juvenile delinquents? Journal of Moral Education, 33, 321–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooker, I., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2013). Is a Bird an Apple? The effect of speaker labeling accuracy on infants’ word learning, imitation, and helping behaviors. Infancy, 18, 46–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broome, J. (1990). Fairness. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 91, 87–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1986). The changing functions of friends in childhood: A Neo-Sullivanian perspective. In V. J. Derlega & B. A. Winstead (Eds.), Friendship and social interaction (pp. 41–62). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cima, M., Tonnaer, F., & Hauser, M. D. (2010). Psychopaths know right from wrong but don’t care. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 59–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clément, F., Koenig, M., & Harris, P. L. (2004). The ontogenesis of trust in testimony. Mind and Language, 19, 360–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colby, A., & Damon, W. (1992). Some do care: Contemporary lives of moral commitment. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colby, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1987). The measurement of moral judgment, Vol. 1: Theoretical foundations and research validation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corriveau, K. H., Harris, P. L., Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Elliott, L., et al. (2009). Young children’s trust in their mother’s claims: longitudinal links with attachment security in infancy. Child Development, 80, 750–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, E., & O’Dougherty Wright, M. (2007). The impact of childhood psychological maltreatment on interpersonal schemas and subsequent experiences of relationship aggression. Journal of Emotional Abuse, 7, 93–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiYanni, C., Nini, D., Rheel, W., & Livelli, A. (2012). “I won’t trust you if I think you’re trying to deceive me”: Relations between selective trust, theory of mind, and imitation in early childhood. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 354–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fairness. (2014). In dictionary.cambridge.org. Retrieved April 10, 2014, from http://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/british/fairness_1?q=fairness

  • Flanagan, C. (2003). Trust, identity, and civic hope. Applied Developmental Science, 7, 165–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furman, W., & Bierman, K. L. (1984). Children’s conceptions of friendship: A multimethod study of developmental changes. Developmental Psychology, 20, 925–931.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garmon, L. C., Basinger, K., Gregg, V. R., & Gibbs, J. C. (1996). Gender differences in stage and expression of moral judgment. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 418–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, J. C. (2010). Beyond the conventionally moral [review of nice is not enough]. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 106–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, J. C. (2014). Moral development and reality: Beyond the theories of Kohlberg, Hoffman, and Haidt. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, J. C., Arnold, K. D., Morgan, R. L., Schwartz, E. S., Gavaghan, M. P., & Tappan, M. B. (1984). Construction and validation of a multiple-choice measure of moral reasoning. Child Development, 55, 527–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, J. C., Basinger, K. S., Grimes, R. L., & Snarey, J. R. (2007). Moral judgment development across cultures: Revisiting Kohlberg’s universality claims. Developmental Review, 27, 443–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, J. C., Basinger, K. S., & Fuller, D. (1992). Moral maturity: Measuring the development of sociomoral reasoning. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, J. C., Potter, G. B., Goldstein, A. P., & Brentro, L. K. (1996). Equipping youth with mature moral judgment. Reclaiming Children and Youth, 5, 156–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregg, V., Gibbs, J. C., & Basinger, K. S. (1994). Patterns of developmental delay in moral judgment by male and female delinquents. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 538–553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, V. (2010). The road: Short fiction and articles (Robert and Elizabeth Chandler, Olga Mukovnikova, Trans.). London: Quercus, Maclehose Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamann, K., Bender, J., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Meritocratic sharing is based on collaboration in 3-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 50, 121–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450, 557–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (2007). Trust. Developmental Science, 10, 135–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, E. (1999). The right to be human: A biography of Abraham Maslow (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hooker, B. (2005). Fairness. Ethical Theory and Moral Practice, 8, 329–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holcombe, R. G. (1983). Applied fairness theory: Comment. The American Economic Review, 73, 1153–1156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2004). Empathy and offending: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9, 441–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2007). Examining the relationship between low empathy and self-reported offending. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 12, 265–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. (2013). The human spark: The science of human development. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahn, P. H., & Turiel, E. (1988). Children’s conceptions of trust in the context of social expectations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 34, 403–419.

    Google Scholar 

  • Killen, M., Mulvey, K. L., Richardson, C., Jampol, N., & Woodward, A. (2011). The accidental transgressor: Morally-relevant theory of mind. Cognition, 119, 197–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. (2005). The role of social cognition in early trust. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 457–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, M. A., Clement, F., & Harris, P. L. (2004). Trust in testimony: Children’s use of true and false statements. Psychological Science, 15, 694–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1971). From is to ought: How to commit the naturalistic fallacy and get away with it in the study of moral development. In T. Mischel (Ed.), Cognitive development and epistemology. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays on moral development, Vol. 2: The psychology of moral development. San Francisco: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Korthals, M. (1994). Over de rol van emoties bij het moreel redeneren. Gibbs’ mislukte poging tot integratie van de theorie van Kohlberg met die van Hoffman. [The role of emotions in moral reasoning Gibbs’s failed attempt to integrate the theories of Kohlberg and Hoffman]. Comenius, 14, 38–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landrum, A. R., Mills, C. M., & Johnston, A. M. (2013). When do children trust the expert? Benevolence information influences children’s trust more than expertise. Developmental Science, 16, 622–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lapsley, D. K. (2006). Moral stage theory. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 37–66). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leenders, I., & Brugman, D. (2005). The moral/nonmoral domain shift in relation to delinquent behavior in adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 65–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, A. J., Lewis, C., Pala, F. C., Wong, K., & Berridge, D. (2013). Social-cognitive processes in preschoolers’ selective trust: Three cultures compared. Developmental Psychology, 49, 579–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, S. (2004). Sentimental rules: On the natural foundations of moral judgment. Oxford: University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L. (2000). The promise and limitations of the moral self construct. Presidential address delivered at the annual meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Montreal, Canada

    Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L. (2004). Reflections on the moral self construct. In D. K. Lapsley & D. Narvaez (Eds.), Moral development, self, and identity (pp. 111–132). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L. P., & Herman, S. (1982). Behavioral disordered children’s conceptions of moral, conventional, and personal issues. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 10, 411–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L. P., & Nucci, M. S. (1982). Children’s responses to moral and social conventional transgressions in free play settings. Child Development, 53, 1337–1342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malti, T., Gasser, L., & Buchmann, M. (2009). Emotion attributions and moral reasoning of aggressive and prosocial children. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 90–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mascaro, O., & Sperber, D. (2009). The moral, epistemic, and mind reading components of children’s vigilance towards deception. Cognition, 112, 367–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mills, C. M. (2013). Knowing when to doubt: Developing a critical stance when learning from others. Developmental Psychology, 49, 404–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nas, C., Brugman, D., & Koops, W. (2008). Measuring self-serving cognitive distortions with the how i think questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24, 181–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Over, H., Carpenter, M., Spears, R., & Gattis, M. (2013). Children selectively trust individuals who have imitated them. Social Development, 22, 215–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pasquini, E., Corriveau, K. H., Koenig, M., & Harris, P. L. (2007). Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1216–1226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Power, F. C., Higgins, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1989). Lawrence Kohlberg’s approach to moral education. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, M. W., Skoe, E. E., & Arnold, M. L. (2004). The development of care reasoning in later adolescence: A longitudinal analysis. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 139–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawls, J. (1999). A theory of Justice (Revised ed.). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescher, N. (2002). Fairness: Theory and practice of distributive justice. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rest, J. R., Narvaez, D., Thoma, S. J., & Bebeau, M. J. (1999). Postconventional moral thinking: A Neo-Kohlbergian approach. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ribeaud, D., & Eisner, M. (2010). Are moral disengagement, neutralization techniques, and self-serving cognitive distortions the same? Developing a unified scale of moral neutralization of aggression. International Journal of Conflict and Violence, 4, 298–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotenberg, K. J. (1980). “A promise kept, a promise broken”: Developmental bases of trust. Child Development, 51, 614–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotenberg, K. J. (1984). Sex differences in children’s trust in peers. Sex Roles, 12, 953–957.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotenberg, K. J. (1986). Same-sex patterns and sex differences in the trust-value basis of children’s friendship. Sex Roles, 15, 613–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotenberg, K. J. (1994). Loneliness and interpersonal trust. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 152–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotenberg, K. J. (2010). The conceptualization of interpersonal trust: A basis, domain, and target framework. In K. J. Rotenberg (Ed.), Interpersonal trust during childhood and adolescence (pp. 2–27). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rotenberg, K. J., Addis, N., Betts, L. R., Corrigan, A., Fox, C., Hobson, Z., et al. (2010). The relation between trust beliefs and loneliness during early childhood, middle childhood, and adulthood. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 1086–1100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotenberg, K. J., Betts, L. R., & Moore, J. (2013). The relation between early adolescents’ trust beliefs in peers and reactions to peer provocation: Attributions of intention and retaliation. The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development, 174, 450–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotenberg, K. J., MacDonald, K. J., & King, E. V. (2004). The relationship between loneliness and interpersonal trust during middle childhood. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 165, 233–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotenberg, K. J., & Mann, L. (1986). The development of the norm of the reciprocity of self-disclosure and its function in children’s attraction to peers. Child Development, 57, 1349–1357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotenberg, K. J., & Pilipenko, T. A. (1983–1984). Mutuality, temporal consistency, and helpfulness in children's trust in peers. Social Cognition, 2, 235–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so different after all: A cross discipline view of trust. Academy of Management Review, 23, 393–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schonert-Reichl, K. A. (1999). Relations of peer acceptance, friendship, adjustment, and social behavior to moral reasoning during early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 249–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Selman, R. L., & Selman, A. E. (1979). Children’s ideas about friendship: A new theory. Psychology Today, 13, 71–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smetana, J. G. (1981). Preschool children’s conceptions of moral and social rules. Child Development, 52, 1333–1336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smetana, J. G. (1989). Adolescents’ and parents’ reasoning about actual family conflict. Child Development, 60, 105–1067.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smetana, J. (1995). Parenting styles and conceptions of parental authority during adolescence. Child Development, 66, 299–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smetana, J. G., Killen, M., & Turiel, E. (1991). Children’s reasoning about interpersonal and moral conflicts. Child Development, 62, 629–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snarey, J. (1985). The cross-cultural universality of social-moral development: A critical review of Kohlbergian research. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 202–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stams, G. J. M. M., Brugman, D., Dekovic, M., Van Rosmalen, L., van der Laan, P., & Gibbs, J. C. (2006). The moral judgment of juvenile delinquents: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 697–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tisak, M. S. (1995). Domains of social reasoning and beyond. Annals of Child Development, 11, 95–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tisak, M. S., & Turiel, E. (1988). Variation in seriousness of transgressions and children’s moral and conventional concepts. Developmental Psychology, 24, 352–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tisak, M. S., Tisak, J., & Goldstein, S. E. (2006). Aggression, morality, and delinquency: A social cognitive perspective. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 611–632). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trust. (2014). In dictionary.cambridge.org. Retrieved April 10, 2014, from http://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/british/trust_1?q=trust

  • Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed.). Vol. 3: N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 863–932). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turiel, E. (2006). Thought, emotions, and social interactional processes in moral development. In M. Killen, & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 7–35). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • United Nations. (2014a). Universal declaration of human rights. Retrieved January 29, 2014, from http://www.un.org/en/documents/udhr/index.shtml

  • United Nations. (2014b). The Office of the United Nations High Commissioner for Human Rights (OHCHR). Retrieved January 29, 2014, from http://www.ohchr.org/EN/Issues/Pages/WhatareHumanRights.aspx

  • Uslaner, E. M. (2001, September). Trust as a moral value. Paper presented at the Conference: Social Capital: Interdisciplinary Perspectives. University of Exeter, United Kingdom

    Google Scholar 

  • Uslaner, E. M. (2002). The moral foundations of trust. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Van Boxtel, K., Brugman, D., Van de Schoot, R., & Aleva, A. E. (2013). Moral cognitions and externalizing problem behavior in children: differences between psychiatric disorders. Poster presented at the 8th International Conference on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, Roehampton University, London, 15–17 July 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Velden, F., Brugman, D., Boom, J., & Koops, W. (2010). Moral cognitive processes explaining antisocial behavior in young adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34, 292–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Ijzendoorn, M., & Zwart-Woudstra, H. A. (1995). Adolescents’ attachment representations and moral reasoning. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 156, 359–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Vugt, E., Stams, G. J., Deković, M., Brugman, D., Rutten, E., & Hendriks, J. (2008). Moral development and solo juvenile sex offenders. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 14, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, L. J. (2006). Gender and morality. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 93–115). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, L. J., & Frimer, J. A. (2007). Moral personality of brave and caring exemplars. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 845–860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, L. J., & Hennig, K. H. (1997). Moral development in the broader context of personality. In S. Hala (Ed.), The development of social cognition (pp. 297–327). East Sussex: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F., Evans, A. D., Li, C., Li, Q., Heyman, G., & Lee, K. (2014). The role of honesty and benevolence in children’s judgments of trustworthiness. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37, 257–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel Brugman .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Brugman, D., Out, C., Gibbs, J.C. (2016). Fairness and Trust in Developmental Psychology. In: Kury, H., Redo, S., Shea, E. (eds) Women and Children as Victims and Offenders: Background, Prevention, Reintegration. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-08398-8_10

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-08398-8_10

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-08397-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-08398-8

  • eBook Packages: Law and CriminologyLaw and Criminology (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics